hong kong autism

Upload: adrian-chan

Post on 06-Apr-2018

222 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/3/2019 Hong Kong Autism

    1/13

    Abnormalities in the anterior cingulate cortex associated with

    attentional and inhibitory control deficits: A neurophysiological

    study on children with autism spectrum disorders

    Agnes S. Chan a,b,*, Yvonne M.Y. Han a, Winnie Wing-man Leung a, Connie Leung c,Virginia C.N. Wong c, Mei-chun Cheung d

    a Neuropsychology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, Chinab Integrative Neuropsychological Rehabilitation Center, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, Chinac Department of Paediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, Duchess of Kent Childrens Hospital at Sandy Bay, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, Chinad Institute of Textiles and Clothing, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong SAR, China

    1. Introduction

    Attention and inhibitory processes are important components in executive functions (Denckla, 1996; Lezak, Howieson, &Loring, 2004; Stuss, Binns, Murphy, & Alexander, 2002) that are essential for effective daily living, and are also among the

    most well-researched constructs in cognitive science. Attention is the ability to mindfully and consciously process stimuli

    (Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley, & Yiend, 1997), and includes alerting, orienting, and sustaining attention (Posner &

    DiGirolamo, 1998), as well as attention shifting (Courchesne et al., 1994; Hughes & Russell, 1993; Landry & Bryson, 2004).

    Inhibitory control refers to the suppressing of responses to irrelevant, non-target, or distracting stimuli ( Enticott, Ogloff, &

    Bradshaw, 2006; Friedman & Miyake, 2004; Nigg, 2000). It puts a high demand on coordination including preparing for

    Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (2011) 254266

    A R T I C L E I N F O

    Article history:

    Received 19 February 2010

    Received in revised form 9 March 2010

    Accepted 12 April 2010

    Keywords:

    Anterior cingulate

    Attention

    Inhibitory controlEEG

    Autism

    Children

    A B S T R A C T

    Previous studies showed that the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is activated when

    individuals engage in attention and inhibitory control tasks. The present study examined

    whether ACC activity is associated with behavioral performance of the two tasks. Twenty

    normal and 20 children with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) were subjected to

    neuropsychological assessments on attention and inhibitory control, as well as

    electroencephalography recording. Children with ASD performed significantly worse

    than normal children on attention tasks as shown in their poorer performance on the Digit

    Span test, the greaternumber of Omission Errors on both the Continuous Performance TestII and the Go/No-Go tasks. They also performed significantly worse than normal children

    on inhibitory control tasks as shown by the greater number of False Alarms on the Object

    Recognition and Hong Kong List Learning Test. Their ACCactivities, as indicated by relative

    theta power, were found to be significantly lower than those of normal controls during

    performance of the Go/No-Go task. Depressed ACC activities were further found to be

    significantly associated with poorer performance in attention and inhibition. Clinical

    implications on the use of theta activities in the ACC as an indicator to monitor

    intervention progress in children with ASD were discussed.

    2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    * Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, N.T., Hong Kong SAR, China. Tel.: +852 2609 6654;

    fax: +852 2603 5019.

    E-mail address: [email protected] (A.S. Chan).

    Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

    Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders

    J o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : h t t p : / / e e s . e l s e v i e r . c o m / R A S D / d ef a u l t . a s p

    1750-9467/$ see front matter 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2010.04.007

    mailto:[email protected]://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/17509467http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.04.007http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.04.007http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/17509467mailto:[email protected]
  • 8/3/2019 Hong Kong Autism

    2/13

    responses, monitoring performance, and detecting errors (Barkley, 1997). One of the most prominent brain regions

    implicated in attention and inhibitory control is the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Garavan, Ross, Murphy, Roche, & Stein,

    2002; Menon, Adleman, White, Glover, & Reiss, 2001; Posner & Petersen, 1990; Reischies et al., 2005). A number of

    neuroimaging studies have found increased ACC activity during tasks that involve attention ( Cabeza & Nyberg, 1997;

    Devinsky, Morrell, & Vogt, 1995; Elliott & Dolan, 1998); error detection (Botvinick, Cohen, & Carter, 2004; Carter et al., 2000);

    and response monitoring (Taylor, Stern, & Gehring, 2007). Furthermore, event-related functional magnetic resonance

    imaging (fMRI) studies on healthy participants have also reported altered patterns of ACC and prefrontal activation in slow

    responders during response inhibition tasks (Hester, Fassbender, & Garavan, 2004).From the neurophysiological perspective, studies using electroencephalography (EEG) have widely reported that

    focused attention was closely associated with increased frontal midline theta power in normal individuals ( Cahn &

    Polich, 2006; Pizzagalli, Oakes, & Davidson, 2003), and that frontal midline theta power is reflective of attentional and

    inhibitory processing in adults (Kropotov, Crawford, & Polyakov, 1997) and children (Chan & Leung, 2006; Daoust,

    Limoges, Bolduc, Mottron, & Godbout, 2004; Monastra, Lubar, & Linden, 2001; Monastra et al., 1999 ) patient groups.

    Frontal theta activity has been widely documented to be generated by the medial prefrontal cortex in the area of ACC

    (Asada, Fukuda, Tsunoda, Yamaguchi, & Tonoike, 1999; Ishii et al., 1999; Pizzagalli et al., 2003 ). For example, Gevins,

    Smith, McEvoy, and Yu (1997), using high spatial resolution EEG combined with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI),

    suggested a frontal medial source for the theta in the ACC region. Similarly, Onton, Delorme, and Makeig (2005), using a

    dipole source model, localized the frontal midline theta to the area of dorsal ACC. In addition, electrophysiological study

    also revealed the source of frontal theta activity to be generated in the ACC during working memory task ( Sauseng,

    Hoppe, Klimesch, Gerloff, & Hummel, 2007). Thus, we used the theta index to measure the neurophysiological activity of

    the ACC in the study.While neuroimaging and neurophysiological studies have provided evidence to suggest that the ACC is involved in

    attention and inhibitory control, the association between ACC activities and neuropsychological performance in attention

    and inhibitory control has not been well studied. We thus aimed to examine whether there is an association between the

    two. A patient sample with autism was recruited in the present study to examine the neurophysiological dysfunction in

    ACC and its association with neuropsychological performance. While this patient population is considerably

    heterogeneous in terms of behavioral deficits as well as structural and functional neural abnormalities, it is well

    documented that these individuals have impairments in attention and inhibitory control (Burack, 1994; Goldstein,

    Johnson, & Minshew, 2001; Hazlett et al., 2004; Haznedar et al., 2000; Mundy, 2003; Nyden, Gillberg, Hjelmquist, &

    Heiman, 1999; Schmitz et al., 2006), and abnormalities in the ACC (Bauman & Kemper, 1994; Gomot et al., 2006; Hazlett

    et al., 2004; Haznedar et al., 2006, 1997, 2000; Kana, Keller, Minshew, & Just, 2007; Luna et al., 2002; Schmitz et al., 2006;

    Thakkar et al., 2008).

    Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) consist of a spectrum of neurodevelopmental disorders that are characterized by

    disturbances in communication, poor social skills, stereotyped behaviors, and deficits in memory and executive functions(Gillberg, 1993; Wing, 1997). Autism, Asperger syndrome, and pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified

    (PDD-NOS) are the three main forms of disorders in the spectrum. Autism is the core disorder of ASD, Asperger syndrome is

    diagnosed when the individual does not have significant delay in cognitive and language development, and PDD-NOS is

    diagnosed when the individual does not meet the full criteria for a more specific disorder in the spectrum (Johnson, Myers, &

    Council on Children with Disabilities, 2007). While the cause of ASD is not well understood, it has been suggested that in

    these individuals, abnormalities in their ACC are associated with executive function deficits and repetitive stereotyped

    behaviors (Haznedar et al., 1997; Mundy, 2003; Schmitz et al., 2006). Given the essential roles of attention (Goldstein et al.,

    2001) and inhibition in suppressing irrelevant thoughts and interference during executive tasks (Fernandez-Duque, Baird, &

    Posner, 2000), some researchers further postulated that primary deficits in attentional and inhibitory controls may account

    for the executive dysfunctions that underlie many of the repetitive, stereotyped behaviors in ASD ( Burack, 1994; Goldstein

    et al., 2001; Nyden et al., 1999; Schmitz et al., 2006). Indeed, findings of numerous neuropsychological studies on ASD

    suggested that individuals with ASD have difficulties in response inhibitory control and slow information processing (Bishop

    & Norbury, 2005; Fernandez-Duque et al., 2000; Hazlett et al., 2004; Hughes, Russell, & Robbins, 1994; Nyden et al., 1999;Russell & Jarrold, 1998; Russell, Jarrold, & Hood, 1999; Schmitz et al., 2006 ), even though some studies failed to find such

    evidence (Kleinhans, Akshoomoff, & Delis, 2005; Ozonoff & Jensen, 1999; Ozonoff & Strayer, 1997). Other studies on children

    with ASD have also found self-monitoring impairments associated with attention and inhibitory deficits in executive

    functioning tasks of memory (Hill & Russell, 2002; Russell & Jarrold, 1999), error-correction (Russell & Jarrold, 1998), and in

    tasks that involve suppression of a prepotent response from prior learning (Bishop & Norbury, 2005; Russell et al., 1999).

    Neuroimaging studies, on the other hand, have shown that individuals with ASD showed atypical ACC activation when they

    were performing spatial working memory (Luna et al., 2002), change detection (Gomot et al., 2006), response monitoring

    (Thakkar et al., 2008), and response inhibition (Kana et al., 2007; Schmitz et al., 2006) tasks. In addition, reduced volume and

    decreased glucose metabolism in the anterior cingulate gyrus have also been found in individuals with ASD ( Hazlett et al.,

    2004; Haznedar et al., 2006, 1997, 2000), and postmortem studies of ASD patients have demonstrated abnormalities in the

    cingulate cortex (Bauman & Kemper, 1994).

    These studies suggested that individuals with ASD have structural, physiological, and functional abnormalities in

    the ACC. Based on the reported deficits in attention and inhibitory control and the documented abnormalities of theACC in individuals with ASD, we predicted that the neurophysiological patterns associated with attentional and

    A.S. Chan et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (2011) 254266 255

  • 8/3/2019 Hong Kong Autism

    3/13

    inhibitory deficits in ASD should involve altered patterns of activation in the ACC. Together with findings that frontal

    midline theta power is reflective of attentional and inhibitory processing in children patient groups, it was postulated

    that the deficient attentional and inhibitory control in children with ASD would be associated with theta activity of the

    ACC. This hypothesis was tested by measuring theta activity in the ACC while children with ASD were performing the

    Go/No-Go task that involved both attention and inhibitory control. This task requires participants to focus on a

    computer monitor and make immediate response on Go trials and to inhibit their response on No-Go trials ( Kana

    et al., 2007).

    The purposes of this study were, therefore, to (i) examine the performance of children with ASD in attention andinhibitory control; (ii) examine the activities in their ACC when they were performing the tasks; and (iii) examine the

    association between the two. Since most previous studies were done on adults with ASD, children were examined in this

    study in the hope of extending previous knowledge to the pediatric population. It was hypothesized that children with ASD

    would perform significantly poorer than normal children in attention and inhibition; that under the task condition, children

    with ASD would show different ACC theta activities compared with normal children; and that attention and inhibition

    performance would be associated with ACC theta activities.

    2. Material and methods

    2.1. Participants

    Twenty normal children and 20 children with ASD (17 with autistic disorder, two with Asperger disorder, one with

    pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified), aged 714 years, participated voluntarily in the study. Normalcontrol (NC) children were recruited from local primary schools or from the database of our laboratory. Invitation letters

    were sent to parents through the schools, and all children whose parents indicated an interest to participate were recruited.

    All NC children had negative history of neurological problems or abnormal developmental milestones as reported by the

    parents. Children with ASD were recruited from the Parents Association of Pre-School Handicapped Children in Hong Kong

    or from the subject database of our laboratory. They were previously diagnosed, based on the criteria in the Diagnostic and

    Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edition) (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 2002) or the Autism

    Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) (Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, 2002), by pediatricians of Child Assessment Centres

    in Hong Kong. Diagnosis was further confirmed by a clinical psychologist through standard clinical interview and the

    Childhood Autism Rating Scale (Schopler, Reichler, & Renner, 1998). Table 1 shows the demographic characteristics of the

    children.Both theASD and NC groups were matched on age, t(38) = 1.51,p> .05, general intelligence as measured by the Test

    of Non-verbal Intelligence, 3rd edition (TONI-III) (Brown, Sherbenou, & Johnsen, 1992; t(38) =1.7, p> .05), and had equal

    gender distribution. It should be noted that the male to female ratio of 19:1 was considerably higher than the reported mean

    ratio of 4.3:1 (Fombonne, 2003). As many of the assessment measures used in this study are dependent on motor skills, allchildren recruited were without physical disabilities or reported motor dysfunction.

    2.2. Procedures and materials

    All children were individually administered a neuropsychological battery that included the TONI-III (Brown et al., 1992)

    and neuropsychological measures of attention and inhibition including the Continuous Performance Test II (Conners, 2000),

    Digit Span (forward) test (Hong Kong Education Department & Hong Kong Psychological Society, 1981), Hong Kong List

    Learning Test (Chan, 2006), and Object Recognition Test (Rossion & Pourtois, 2004). The children were also subjected to an

    EEG recording session where EEG was recorded for a 6-min period while they performed the Go/No-Go task. Because of the

    short attention span and aversion to novel tactile stimulation of this particular patient group, a short recording time was

    necessary to ensure that the children could complete the Go/No-Go task, especially with the EEG cap connected to their

    heads. The sequence of the neuropsychological assessment and EEG recording was counter-balanced to avoid order effect.

    The experimental and EEG recording procedures were explained to the children and parents before the experiment started,and all children participated with informed parental consent. The procedure was approved by the Joint CUHK-NTEC Clinical

    Research Ethics Committee.

    Table 1

    Demographic characteristics of the normal controls (NC) and children with autistic spectrum

    disorders (ASD).

    Variable NC (n = 20) ASD (n = 20)

    Mean age (in years) 9.8 (1.88) 10.75 (2.07)

    Gender (male/female) 19/1 19/1

    TONI-III (deviation quotient) 110.7 (17.84) 101.4 (16.83)

    CARS (total score) 31.98 (3.31)

    Note: Standard deviations are in parentheses. CARS = Childhood Autism Rating Scale; TONI-

    III= Test of Non-verbal Intelligence, 3rd edition. The dash indicates that the CARS was notadministered to normal controls.

    A.S. Chan et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (2011) 254266256

  • 8/3/2019 Hong Kong Autism

    4/13

    2.3. Measures

    2.3.1. Test of Non-verbal Intelligence

    The TONI-III (Brown et al., 1992) was administered to assess non-verbal intelligence. This test consists of 45 matrix

    reasoningquestions, givinga rawscore that rangedfrom 0 to 45, whichis then converted to a deviation quotientbased on the

    norms provided in the test manual.

    2.3.2. Digit Span (DS) forwardThis test is a subtest on the Hong Kong version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (Hong Kong Education

    Department & Hong Kong Psychological Society, 1981) on short-term memory and attention, and was used to assess

    attentionin thepresent study. It consists of a list of nine randomnumerals read aloud in sequence by the examiner at therate

    of one numeral per second, starting from two numerals up to nine numerals in a total of eight trials. Participants are asked to

    repeat the numerals after the examiner, in sequence, after each trial. The score is the number of correct trials recalled by the

    participant.

    2.3.3. Hong Kong List Learning Test (HKLLT)

    The HKLLT (Chan, 2006) was used in the study to measure frontal lobe functions including learning strategies,

    organization, and vulnerability to interference. This is an oral test consisting of a randomly organized list of 16 two-word

    Chinese characters presented once during each of the three learning trials. A recognition test consisting of the 16 target

    words and 16 distracters was presented after the 30-min delayed recall trial. The number of False Alarms was recorded as

    intrusion errors, which is commonly a neuropsychological measure of inhibition (Chan, Cheung, Han, et al., 2009;Cummingham, Pliskin, Cassisi, Tsang, & Rao, 1997; De Beni & Palladino, 2004; Mahone, Koth, Cutting, Singer, & Denckla,

    2001; Stuss et al., 1994).

    2.3.4. Object Recognition (OR) Test

    This test was used to measure non-verbal memory in the study. It consisted of 24 line drawings taken from Snodgrass and

    Vanderwart (1980) object database, modified and validatedby Rossion and Pourtois (2004). The line drawings were placed in

    a six by four array displayed on a computer screen for 3 min. Participants were required to memorize the items for a later

    recognition task that consisted of 12 targets mixed with 12 distractors. Incorrect identification of distractors was counted as

    False Alarms, which is a common measure of intrusion and inhibition (Cornoldi & Mammerella, 2006). Scores on this test

    ranged from 0 to 12.

    2.3.5. Continuous Performance Test II

    The CPT-II (Conners, 2000) was administered on the computer and measured sustained attention, impulse control, andinformation processing speed. Participants were required to press a key as quickly as possible in response to letters of the

    alphabet displayed on the computer screen, with the exception of the letter X. Total Omission Errors measured attention;

    Total Commission Errors measured inhibitory control; and information processing efficiency was reflected in the Hit

    Reaction Time.

    2.3.6. Go/No-Go task

    The computerized Go/No-Go task was used to measure sustained attention, impulse control, and information processing

    speed in the study. Children were required to press a key as quickly as possible when a black ball (Go stimulus) appeared on

    the computer screen, and to inhibit their responses when a red ball (No-Go stimulus) appeared. The total testing time was

    6 min and the stimuli were displayed one at a time, in the center of the computer screen, for 500 msin randomorder at a ratio

    of 4:1 (192 black balls:48 red balls) followed by 1000 ms of blank intervals. The Total Omission Errors on Go trials

    measured attention, the Total Commission Errors on No-Go trials measured inhibition, and Hit Reaction Time measured

    information processing efficiency.

    2.3.7. EEG recording

    Allparents and children were briefed on the procedure, andinformed consents were obtainedbefore EEGrecordings were

    taken. EEG data were collected during the Go/No-Go task using an electrode cap with 19 electrode sites (International 10-20

    System) referenced to linked ears. The EEG signal was digitized at 256 Hz with a low pass filter of 30 Hz and impedances

    below 10 kV. EEG data were stored and later displayed on computer, and visually examined for eye movements and muscle

    artifacts. Only data that had at least 1 min of artifact-free data were selected (see John, Prichep, Fridman, & Easton, 1988 for

    discussion of qEEG method) and spectrally processed using the fast Fourier Transformation (FFT) to compute power data for

    the theta band (47.5 Hz).

    2.3.8. EEG data reduction

    EEG data for each participant was first transformed by Excel application before they were imported into the software

    EEGLAB using MatLab 7.1 to capture correct events and epochs. The average reaction time for normal individuals was set ataround 300400 ms (Miller & Low, 2001). This was calculated based on the assumption that normal individuals take around

    A.S. Chan et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (2011) 254266 257

  • 8/3/2019 Hong Kong Autism

    5/13

    90 ms to perceive the appearance of a visual object, followed by cognitive appraisal, which in turn leads to motor action. To

    ensure that the selected epoch would adequately bracket the data corresponding to the inhibition task, the epoch limit was

    set as 50 msas the start and 900 ms as the end. Artifacts in epoched data were then pruned by visual inspection and using the

    rejection method on EEG Plot. All incorrect hits were also deselected. The transformed data were exported into readable

    format for analysis using the NeuroGuide software.

    2.3.9. Relative theta

    EEG data recorded from the 19 electrode sites were averaged to obtain grand means for the anterior (F3, F4, F7, F8, Fz),centrotemporal (C3, C4, T3, T4, T5, T6, Cz), and posterior (P3, P4, O1, O2, Pz) regions for further analysis. Relative power was

    used for analyses on the theta band based on the rationale that (1) relative power measurements tend to give larger

    estimates for the dominant frequency range (Klimesch, 1999), and (2) individual variations are eliminated by computing the

    proportion of an individual frequency band relative to the others (Chan, Sze, & Cheung, 2007).

    2.3.10. Source analysis

    Previous findings implicated the ACC as a generator for frontal midline theta activities in the human brain (Asada et al.,

    1999; Ishii et al., 1999; Pizzagalli et al., 2001). To localize the sources of the theta activities in response to the Go and No-

    Go conditions, we employed low-resolution electromagnetic tomography (LORETA) (Pascual-Marqui et al., 1999; Pascual-

    Marqui, Michel, & Lehmann, 1994). The sources of the theta activities were expressed as three-dimensional cortical current

    density according to the Talairach brain atlas. All EEG data used for FFT were analyzed with LORETA during the two

    experimental (Go, No-Go) conditions.

    2.4. Data analyses

    ASD and NC children were compared on their performance on the neuropsychological measures on attention and

    inhibitory control, which included 9 scores from the DS, HKLLT, OR, CPT-II, and Go/No-Go tasks using independent t-tests. To

    examine the neurophysiological changes in relative theta power in response to the Go and No-Go conditions, a 2 2 3

    (Diagnosis Condition Region) analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures was performed, with Condition (Go,

    No-Go), and Region (anterior, centrotemporal, posterior) as within-subject factors, and Diagnosis (NC, ASD) as between-

    subject factor. This was followed by post hoc F-tests to pinpoint the brain regions showing significant differences in the theta

    band. Relationships between attention, inhibition and EEG activities across the ASD and NC groups were examined using

    Pearson correlation. To examine whether the correlations reflect only basic group differences, subgroup analyses (NC = 20,

    ASD = 20) were done. Given that specific hypotheses were tested and that the number of participants were relatively small,

    we did not adjust the alpha level to avoid lowering the power of the tests.

    3. Results

    3.1. Neuropsychological measures

    The ASD group showed significantly poorer performance on the DS (t=2.98, p< .01) than the normal group, as well as

    significantly higher False Alarms on the OR (t= 4.04,p< .01) and the HKLLT (t= 2.76,p< .01). For the Go/No-Go task, the ASD

    and NC groups did not differ significantly in the number of Total Commission Errors (t= 1.91, n.s.) during the No-Go trials.

    Table 2

    Mean performance and standard deviation on the attention and inhibitory control of children in the NC and ASD groups.

    Measures NC (n = 20) ASD (n = 20) t-Value

    M (SD) M (SD)

    DSForward 12.79 (1.85) 10.17 (2.62) 2.98**

    HKLLT

    False Alarms .35 (.59) 4.05 (5.82) 2.76**

    OR

    False Alarms .05 (.22) 3.05 (3.32) 4.04**

    Go/No-Go

    Hit Reaction Time (ms) 374.7 (61.8) 425.2 (145) 1.43

    Omission Errors 4.6 (4.58) 16.5 (18.92) 2.73**

    Commission Errors 8.9 (5.56) 12.8(7.23) 1.91

    CPT

    Hit Reaction Time (ms) 412.42 (66.3) 479.44 (130.6) 1.92

    Omission Errors 3.8 (1.33) 17.76 (23.07) 2.46*

    Commission Errors 23.15 (5.66) 23.18 (5.41) .01

    The mean performance of CPT task was computed based on 20 NC and 17 ASD children.*

    p< .05.** p< .01.

    A.S. Chan et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (2011) 254266258

  • 8/3/2019 Hong Kong Autism

    6/13

    Independent t-test also showed no significant difference between the two groups on Hit Reaction Time (t= 1.44, n.s.) on the

    Go/No-Go task. The ASD group, however, produced significantly more Total Omission Errors (t= 2.73, p< .01) than the NC

    group during the Go trials. For the CPT-II, three outliers in the ASD group were excluded in the analysis because of the

    unreasonably large amount of Omission Errors (>

    3SD) suggesting that they were not performing the test according to theinstructions. Results on the CPT-II were consistent with those on the Go/No-Go task, in that no significant difference was

    found between the ASD and NC groups on Hit Reaction Time (t= 1.92, p> .05) and Total Commission Errors (t= .01, p> .05).

    The ASD group, however, showed significantly higher Total Omission Errors (t= 2.46, p< .05) than the normal group

    (Table 2). While the children with ASD showed significantly poorer performance in various attention and inhibition tasks

    Fig. 1. Topographic maps demonstrating the mean values of relative theta power, with the frontal regions at the top of each map, are shown for normal

    children (NC), and children with autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) during (a) Go and (b) No-Go conditions.

    Fig.2. (a) Go condition: Children with autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) showedsignificantly lower mean relative theta values (%)than normalcontrol (NC)

    in the anterior region, butnot in thecentrotemporal andposterior regions. (b) No-Go condition: Children with ASDdemonstrated significantly lower meanrelative theta values in the anterior and centrotemporal regions, but not in the posterior region. *p< .05.

    A.S. Chan et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (2011) 254266 259

  • 8/3/2019 Hong Kong Autism

    7/13

    than normal controls, it should be noted the ASD group also showed large within-group variability, violating the assumption

    of homogeneity of variances. The set of t-test results with homogeneity not assumed was thus used.

    3.2. Neurophysiological measures

    Maps showing the regional relative theta power of the ASD and NC groups are presented in Fig. 1. Visual examination

    showed that relative theta power was lower across multiple channels in the ASD group. Repeated measures ANOVA results

    showed no significant interaction effect of Diagnosis

    Condition [F(1,38) = 1.38, p>

    .05] and of Diagnosis

    Region[F(2,76) = 2.03, p> .05]. The results, however, indicated a significant Condition Region interaction for the theta band

    [F(2,76) = 5.28, p< .05], and a significant between-subject effect [F(1,38) = 6.91, p< .05]. For the Go condition, tests of

    simple effects demonstrated that the ASD group showed significant decrease in relative theta power than normal children in

    the anterior [F(1,38) = 14.41,p< .05] but not the centrotemporal [F(1,38) = 4.03,p> .05] and posterior [F(1,38) = .56,p> .05]

    brain regions. For the No-Go condition, the ASD group showed significant decrease in relative theta power in the anterior

    [F(1,38) = 5.88,p< .05] and thecentrotemporal [F(1,38) = 8.61,p< .05] but not the posterior [F(1,38) = 3.08,p> .05] regionas

    compared to the normal control group (Fig. 2).

    3.3. Localization of theta activity

    To examine whether the ASD group differed from the normal group in the sources of the theta activity, source analysis

    was conducted using the LORETA voxel-by-voxel t-value to compare theta activities of the ASD and NC groups on the Go

    and No-Go conditions. Compared with the normal controls, results revealed that the ASD group showed significantlyreduced theta activities in the ACC during the Go [Brodmann area 24; t= 4.83 (p< .05)] and No-Go [Brodmann area 24,

    t= 2.61 (p< .05)] conditions. In addition to the reduced activation in ACC for the No-Go condition, a more significant

    underactivation was also found in the Precuneus [Brodmann area 7, t= 3.01 (p< .05)] (Fig. 3).

    Fig. 3. Graphical representation of the LORETA t-statistics comparing the (a) Go-related and (b) No-Go-related activations of the ASD and NC groups. Red

    color indicates thelocationof significantlyreduced electrical activity in thebrainof theASD group as compared to thenormal controls.The location of voxel

    is definedby theTalairach coordinates (X, Y,Z) andthe black arrowsindicatethe centerof differencein ACCactivity(Brodmann area 24). (For interpretationof the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of the article.)

    A.S. Chan et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (2011) 254266260

  • 8/3/2019 Hong Kong Autism

    8/13

    3.4. Association between attention, inhibition and theta activities in the ACC

    Given that children with ASD were found to differ from normal children in their task-related ACC theta activities as

    indicated by the anterior relative theta power, and in measures of attention and inhibitory control as indicated by

    performance on the neuropsychological assessments, we examined the association between the two using Pearson

    correlations on the whole group of children and separately for the ASD and normal subgroups. Anterior theta power duringthe Go condition for the combined group was significantly correlated with DS (r= .40, p< .05), OR False Alarms (r=.42,

    p< .01), and HKLLT False Alarms (r=.32,p< .05); and anterior theta power during the No-Go condition was significantly

    correlated with OR False Alarms (r=.50, p< .001) (Table 3). In the subgroup analysis, findings on the ASD group were

    consistent in that anterior theta power during the Go condition was significantly correlated with DS (r= .59, p< .05), and

    anterior theta power during the No-Go condition was significantly correlated with OR False Alarms (r=.47, p< .05).

    Although no significant association between OR False Alarms (r=.26, p.> .05) and HKLLT False Alarms (r=.19, p> .05)

    with anterior relative theta power was found as in the combined-group analysis, this was likely to be due to the lower power

    as a result of the smaller number of participants in the subgroup analysis. No significant association of the

    neurophysiological and neuropsychological responses was found in the NC group (Table 4).

    4. Discussion and conclusions

    This study investigated whether neuropsychological deficits in attention and inhibitory control were associated withtheir neurophysiological activity on a group of children with ASD during their performance of such tasks. Results on the

    Table 3

    Correlations between anterior relative theta power, IQ, and mean performance on neuropsychological tests ( n = 40).

    Go

    Anterior

    theta

    No-Go

    Anterior

    theta

    TONI-III Go/ No-Go

    Omission

    Errors

    CPT

    Omission

    Errors

    DS

    Forward

    OR

    False

    Alarms

    HKLLT

    False

    Alarms

    GoAnterior theta 1.00

    No-GoAnterior theta .72*** 1.00

    TONI-III .21 .13 1.00

    Go/No-GoOmission Errors .10 .22 .37* 1.00CPTOmission Errors .09 .21 .34* .75*** 1.00

    DSForward .40* .26 .40* .54** .56** 1.00

    ORFalse Alarms .42** .50*** .35* .58*** .61*** .61*** 1.00

    HKLLTFalse Alarms .32* .08 .34* .66*** .84*** .71*** .60*** 1.00* p< .05.** p< .01.*** p< .001.

    Table 4

    Correlations between anterior relative theta power, IQ, and mean performance on neuropsychological tests for the ASD and NC subgroups.

    Go

    Anterior

    theta

    No-Go

    Anterior

    theta

    TONI-III Go/No-Go

    Omission

    Errors

    CPT

    Omission

    Errors

    DS

    Forward

    OR

    False

    Alarms

    HKLLT

    False

    Alarms

    ASD (n = 20)

    GoAnterior theta 1.00

    No-GoAnterior theta .70*** 1.00

    TONI-III .22 .08 1.00

    Go/No-GoOmission Errors .01 .05 .43 1.00

    CPTOmission Errors .26 .01 .36 .73* 1.00

    DSForward .59* .10 .27 .55 .62 1.00

    ORFalse Alarms .26 .47* .36 .49* .39 .57 1.00

    HKLLTFalse Alarms .19 .09 .40 .63** .87*** .77** .48* 1.00

    NC (n = 20)

    GoAnterior theta 1.00

    No-GoAnterior theta .61** 1.00

    TONI-III .40 .26 1.00

    Go/No-GoOmission Errors .09 .27 .04 1.00

    CPTOmission Errors .19 .34 .14 .32 1.00DSForward .42 .04 .44 .24 .49 1.00

    ORFalse Alarms .24 .11 .10 .13 .04 .19 1.00

    HKLLTFalse Alarms .16 .01 .02 .02 .18 .26 .14 1.00* p< .05.** p< .01.*** p< .001.

    A.S. Chan et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (2011) 254266 261

  • 8/3/2019 Hong Kong Autism

    9/13

    neuropsychological performance confirmed the hypothesis that children with ASD performed poorer than normal children

    on measures of attention including significantly poorer performance on the DS test, and committed significantly more

    Omission Errors on CPT-II and the Go trial of the Go/No-Go tasks. They also performed significantly poorer on measures of

    inhibition including committing significantly more False Alarms on the HKLLT and the OR. Findings on the

    neurophysiological activities also confirmed the hypothesis that under the task condition, children with ASD would

    show different ACC theta activities. Children with ASD in the present study exhibited lower relative theta activities in the

    anterior brain region and less activation in the ACC, localized by LORETA estimation, in both Go and No-Go conditions

    compared with normal controls. The hypothesis that attention and inhibition performance would be associated with ACCtheta activities was also confirmed. Examination across the NC and ASD groups showed that depressed theta activities in the

    ACC during the Go condition (i.e. attention) was significantly associated with poorer performance in attention measures

    including the DS, HKLLT False Alarm and OR False Alarm. Depressed theta activities in the ACC during the No-Go condition

    (i.e. inhibition) were also found to be significantly associated with poorer performance in the inhibition measure of OR False

    Alarm. Subgroup analysis showed that a similar pattern was also observed in children with ASD.

    The present findings are in line with previous findings that individuals with ASD had deficits in attentional (Goldstein

    et al., 2001; Nyden et al., 1999; Sturm, Fernell, & Gillberg, 2004) and inhibitory controls (Bishop & Norbury, 2005; Chan,

    Cheung, Han, et al., 2009;Chan, Cheung, Sze, Leung,& Shi, 2009;Chan,Cheung, Tsui, Sze, & Shi, 2009; Fernandez-Duque et al.,

    2000; Hazlett et al., 2004;Hughes et al., 1994; Russell & Jarrold, 1998; Russell et al., 1999). In the present study, children with

    ASD performed comparably with normal children in Commission Errors but poorly in Omission Errors on both the Go/No-Go

    and CPT-II tasks, suggesting that they were within the normal range in simple inhibitory control but showed impairments in

    attention. These findings are in line with prior studies that reported attention deficits in children with ASD (Bishop &

    Norbury, 2005; Nyden et al., 1999), resulting in increased rate of Omission Errors. The results are also consistent withprevious findings that inhibition deficit in ASD were not pervasive but depended on the complexity of the task ( Kana et al.,

    2007). As suggested by some researchers, response inhibition does not occur in isolation, but is a key executive process that

    governs the coordination of other facets of processing so that a response is inhibited at just the right time ( Denckla, 2002;

    Kana et al., 2007; Nyden et al., 1999). This may explain why children with ASD in the present study were relatively

    unimpaired in the simple Go/No-Go and CPT-II tasks, but showed impaired performance in intrusion and False Alarms on the

    more complex HKLLT and OR tasks which required multiple executive functions (Stuss et al., 1994).

    Findings in the present study on the association between ACC dysfunction and cognitive impairments were also

    consistent with reported findings for adults with ASD (Haznedar et al., 2000; Henderson et al., 2006; Mundy, 2003; Shafritz,

    Dichter, Baranek, & Belger, 2008). Children with ASD in the present study demonstrated significantly lower frontal midline

    relative theta power, indicating reduced activation of the ACC, when performing the attention- and inhibition-demanding

    Go/No-Go task. The ACC has been found to play a central role in several complex functions associated with attention ( Kok,

    Ridderinkhof, & Ullsperger, 2006; Schmitz et al., 2006) and response inhibition, such as the monitoring of task performance

    (Botvinick et al., 2004; Bush, Luu, & Posner, 2000). These relatively complex information processing tasks are those thatindividuals with ASD have difficulty with (Minshew, Meyer, & Goldstein, 2002). In addition to the reduced signal estimated

    from the ACC, children with ASD in our study also showed significantly depressed relative theta power in the parietal region

    during the response inhibition task of the No-Go trials compared with normal children. These results are consistent with

    previous findings that suggested significant parietal involvement in response inhibition (Kana et al., 2007; Liddle, Kiehl, &

    Smith, 2001; Menon et al., 2001). Specifically, the parietal region, in conjunction with the ACC, has been shown to play a

    fundamental role in error detection (Carter et al., 1998), response conflict (Braver, Barch, Gray, Molfese, & Snyder, 2001; Van

    Veen, Cohen, Botvinick, Stenger, & Carter, 2001), and visual-spatial alerting and orienting (Corbetta, Kincade, Ollinger,

    McAvoy, & Shulman, 2000; Coull, Frith, Frackowiak, & Grasby, 1996). Hence, the depressed relative theta power over the

    parietal andACC regions duringthe response inhibition task of the No-Go trials suggests problem with inhibitory control in

    children with ASD in our study.

    It is worth noting that the depressed theta power found in children with ASD in the present study is in contrast with prior

    studies on adults which showed greater theta power (Daoust et al., 2004; Murias, Webb, Greenson, & Dawson, 2007). It has

    been well documented that resting theta power decreases with age ( Gasser, Jennen-Steinmetz, Sroka, Verleger, & Mocks,1988; Gasser, Verleger, Bacher, & Sroka, 1988; Harmony et al., 1990; Somsen, Van-Klooster, Van-der-Molen, & Van-Leeuwen,

    1997), and the mature brain is characterized by a reduction in theta power as compared to the less developed brain in

    younger children (Klimesch, 1999; Schmid, Tirsch, & Reitmeir, 1997) and in individuals with educational problems (Gasser,

    Rousson, & Gasser, 2003; Harmony et al., 1995), learning disabilities (Byring, Salmi, Sainio, & Orn, 1991; Harmony et al.,

    1995; Pinkerton, Watson, & McClelland, 1989) and neurologicaldisorders (Ahnet al., 1980; Coben, Clarke, Hudspeth, & Barry,

    2008; John et al., 1983). The higher theta power in adults with ASD is akin to the less developed brains of younger children,

    suggesting a lag in brain development which may be associated with their pervasive cognitive impairments. Along this line

    of thinking, one would expect that children with ASD would also show elevated theta power compared to normal children.

    The findings of depressed theta power in this study seem to suggest that such maturation lag is not yet evident in pre-

    adolescence children with ASD. However, it should also be noted that in children, theta power may not yet be functionally

    differentiated from other frequency ranges, in particular the alpha (Klimesch, 1999), and thus the decrease in theta rhythm

    observed in the ASD group might actually be due to a decrease in other normal rhythms identified in children.

    What appears to be an important extension of previous studies is that in the present study, it was found that depressedneurophysiological activities in the ACC were significantly associated with poorer performance in attention and inhibition.

    A.S. Chan et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (2011) 254266262

  • 8/3/2019 Hong Kong Autism

    10/13

    This provided some empirical evidence to support the notion that functional abnormality in the ACC may underlie the

    deficits in attentional and inhibitory control in children with ASD. This relationship, in addition to increasing the knowledge

    of the neurophysiological basis of behavioral dysfunction associated with ASD, may also have clinical implications for

    intervention for children with ASD. As theta activities in the ACC are found to be associated with inhibitory deficits in

    children with ASD, this EEG marker may be used as a quick and objective indicator to monitor the progress of children with

    ASD undergoing intervention programs. With this quick and objective method, clinicians would have an alternative than to

    put these children through lengthy neuropsychological assessments commonly available in most clinics, which may be

    difficult for this patient group. This is especially useful for non-verbal children with ASD, as verbal neuropsychological testsare not applicable to this patient group. However, the fact that clinics may not have ready access to EEG equipment may pose

    a limitation on the more widespread use of this method. Another advantage of using this EEG marker over the use of

    conventional neuropsychological tests is that it is less time intensive, on one hand making it more acceptable for children

    with ASD, and on the other reducing the time involvement of professional psychologists which in turn may reduce the cost of

    intervention for patients. Apart from using this EEG marker in treatment monitoring, the possibility of developing

    interventions to improve ACC functioning may also be another research direction on children with ASD ( Chan, Cheung, Sze,

    et al., 2009; Chan, Cheung, Tsui, et al., 2009).

    While there are some interesting observations in the present study that suggested an association between attention,

    inhibitory control and neurophysiological activities, the following should be noted when interpreting the data. First, it

    should noted that since the ADOS and ADI-R are not routinely used for diagnosis of ASD in Hong Kong due to the lack of

    locally validated versions, not all children with ASD in the present study were diagnosed using this gold standard. Second,

    the measures used, i.e. intrusion errors, were relatively simple and indirect measures of inhibitory control ( De Beni &

    Palladino, 2004). In addition, the theta power associated with the Go and No-Go conditions was highly correlated in thepresent study, which may suggest possible involvement of a common underlying function rather than differences in

    inhibition and attention. Further research with other inhibitory control measures is necessary before we can establish for

    certain the relationship between intrusion error, inhibitory control, and ACC activities. Third, the use of the non-verbal TONI-

    III as a surrogate measure for general intelligence has some limitations: the absence of a measure of verbal intelligence may

    introduce a potential confound for task performance especially in the word learning test; andthe use of only non-verbal IQ to

    represent general intelligence may make the participants appear more similar than they actually are. Fourth, while all

    children did not have physical disabilities or motor dysfunctions, the lack of control on the childrens motor abilities should

    be noted as many of the assessments involved motor skills. Finally, caution is raised towards the generalization of the

    findings due to the relatively small sample-size; the non-random selection of the children; and for children with ASD the

    large within-group variations in performance, the overrepresentation of high-functioning children, the young age-group,

    and the high ratio of boys to girls making it difficult to draw conclusions on girls. When interpreting the data, these

    limitations should be borne in mind. Given the potentially useful clinical application of the neurophysiological measure of

    frontal midline theta power, further studies are warrantedto examine the use of this measure on individuals with ASDfrom alarger age range and at different levels of functioning.

    Acknowledgments

    This research was supported by the research grant CUHK 440407 from the Hong Kong Research Grants Council. We thank

    the Parents Association of Pre-School Handicapped Children in Hong Kong for their assistance in recruiting participants.

    References

    Ahn, H., Prichep, L., John, E. R., Baird, H., Trepetin, M., & Kaye, H. (1980). Developmental equations reflect brain dysfunctions. Science, 210, 12591262.American Psychiatric Association. (2002). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.Asada, H., Fukuda, Y., Tsunoda, S., Yamaguchi, M., & Tonoike, M. (1999). Frontal midline theta rhythms reflect alternative activation of prefrontal cortex and

    anterior cingulated cortex in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 274, 2932.Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65

    94.

    Bauman, M. L., & Kemper, T. L. (1994). Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in autism. In M. L. Bauman & T. L. Kemper (Eds.), The neurobiology of autism (pp.119145). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press.

    Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2005). Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symtomatology. 2: Responseinhibition. Autism, 9, 2943.

    Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 539546.Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Gray, J. R., Molfese, D. L., & Snyder, A.(2001). Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: Effects offrequency, inhibition and errors.

    Cerebral Cortex, 11, 825836.Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J., & Johnsen, S. K. (1992). Test of nonverbal intelligence: A language-free measure of cognitive ability (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.Burack, J. A. (1994). Selective attention deficits in persons with autism: Preliminary evidence of an inefficient lens. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 535543.Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in the anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 215222.Byring, R. F., Salmi, T. K., Sainio, K. O., & Orn, H. P. (1991). EEG in children with spelling disabilities. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 79, 247

    255.Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (1997). Imaging cognition: An empirical review of PET studies with normal subjects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 126.Cahn, B. R., & Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 180211.

    Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Botvinick, M. M., Noll, D., & Cohen, J. D. (1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection and the online monitoring ofperformance. Science, 280, 747749.

    A.S. Chan et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (2011) 254266 263

  • 8/3/2019 Hong Kong Autism

    11/13

    Carter, C. S., Macdonald, A. M., Botvinick, M., Ross, L. L., Stenger, V. A., Noll, D., et al. (2000). Parsing executive processes: Strategic vs evaluative functions of theanterior cingulate cortex. PNAS, 97(4), 19441948.

    Chan, A. S. (2006). Hong Kong List Learning test (2nd ed.). Hong Kong: Department of Psychology and Clinical Psychology Centre, The Chinese University of HongKong.

    Chan,A. S.,Cheung,M. C., Han, Y.M. Y., Sze, S.L., Leung, W.W., Man, H.S., etal. (2009). Executive functiondeficits andneural discordance inchildren withautismspectrum disorders. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120, 11071115.

    Chan, A. S., Cheung, M. C., Sze, S. L., Leung, W. W., & Shi, D. (2009). An herbal nasal drop enhanced frontal and anterior cingulate cortex activity. Evidence-basedComplementary and Alternative Medicine. Advance Access published online December 8, 2009, eCAM, doi:10.1093/ecam/nep198.

    Chan, A. S., Cheung, M. C.,Tsui,W. J., Sze,S. L.,& Shi, D. (2009). Dejian MindBody Intervention on depressive mood of community-dwelling adults:A randomizedcontrolled trial. Evidence-based Complementary and Alternative Medicine. Advance Access published online May 27, 2009, eCAM, doi:10.1093/ecam/nep043.

    Chan, A. S., & Leung, W. W. M. (2006). Differentiating autistic children with quantitative encephalography: A 3-month longitudinal study. Journal of ChildNeurology, 21, 391399.

    Chan, A. S.,Sze, S. L.,& Cheung, M. (2007). Quantitative electroencephalographic profilesfor children with autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychology, 21(1), 7481.

    Coben, R., Clarke, A. R., Hudspeth, W., & Barry, R. J. (2008). EEG power and coherence in autism spectrum disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119, 10021009.Conners, C. K. (2000). Conners Continuous Performance Test II. ON: Multi-Health System.Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., McAvoy, M. P., & Shulman, G. L. (2000). Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior

    parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 292297.Cornoldi, C., & Mammerella, N. (2006). Intrusion errors in visuospatial working memory performance. Memory, 14, 176188.Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S., & Grasby, P. M. (1996). A frontal-parietal network for rapid visual information processing: A PET study of sustained

    attention and working memory. Neuropsychologia, 34, 10851095.Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., Akshoomoff, N. A., Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Lincoln, A. J., et al. (1994). Impairment in shifting attention in autistic and

    cerebellar patients. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 848865.Cummingham, J. M.,Pliskin,N. H.,Cassisi,J. E.,Tsang, B.,& Rao, S. M. (1997). Relationshipbetween confabulation andmeasures of memoryand executive function.

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 867877.Daoust, A.-M., Limoges, E.,Bolduc, C.,Mottron, L., & Godbout, R. (2004). EEGspectral analysis of wakefulness and REMsleep in high functioning autistic spectrum

    disorders. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115, 13681373.

    De Beni, R., & Palladino, P. (2004). Decline in working memory updating through ageing: Intrusion error analyses. Memory, 12, 7589.Denckla, M. B. (1996). Biologicalcorrelates of learning and attention: What is relevantto learning disability and attention-deficit hyperactivitydisorder?Journal of

    Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 17, 114119.

    Denckla, M. B. (2002). The behavior rating inventory of executive function: Commentary. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 304306.Devinsky, O., Morrell, M. J., & Vogt, B. A. (1995). Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain, 118, 279306.Elliott, R., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Activation of different anterior cingulate foci in association with hypothesis testing and response selection. Neuroimage, 8, 1729.Enticott, P. G., Ogloff, J. R. P., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2006). Associations between laboratory measures of executive inhibitory control and self-reported impulsivity.

    Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 285294.Fernandez-Duque, D., Baird, J. A., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Executive attention and metacognitive regulation. Consciousness and Cognition, 9, 288307.Fombonne, E. (2003). Epidemiological surveys of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders: An update. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 33,

    365382.Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental

    Psychology: General, 133, 101135.Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., Murphy, K., Roche, R. A. P., & Stein, E. A. (2002). Dissociable executive functions in the dynamic control of behavior: Inhibition, error

    detection, and correction. Neuroimage, 17(4), 18201829.Gasser, T., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., Sroka, L., Verleger, R., & Mocks, J. (1988). Development of the EEG for school-age children and adolescents: II. Topography.

    Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 69, 100109.

    Gasser, T., Rousson, V., & Gasser, U. S. (2003). EEG power and coherence in children with educational problems. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 20, 273282.Gasser, T., Verleger, R., Bacher, P., & Sroka, L. (1988). Development of the EEG for school-age children and adolescents: I. Analysis of band power.

    Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 69, 9199.

    Gevins, A.,Smith, M. E.,McEvoy, L., & Yu, D. (1997). High-resolution EEG mapping of cortical activation related to working memory: Effects of task difficulty, typeof processing, and practice. Cerebral Cortex, 7, 374385.

    Gillberg, C. (1993). Autism and related behaviors. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 37, 343372.Goldstein, G., Johnson, C. R., & Minshew, N. J. (2001). Attentional processes in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 433440.Gomot, M., Bernard, F. A., Davis, M. H., Belmonte, M. K., Ashwin, C., Bullmore, E. T., et al. (2006). Change detection in children with autism: An auditory event-

    related fMRI study. Neuroimage, 29, 475484.Harmony, T., Hinojosa, G., Marosi, E., Becker, J., Rodriguez, M., Reyes, A., et al. (1990). Correlation between EEG spectral parameters and educational evalua tion.

    International Journal of Neuroscience, 54, 147155.Harmony, T., Marosi, E., Becker, J., Rodriguez, M., Reyes, A., Fernandez, T., et al. (1995). Longitudinal quantitative EEG study of children with different

    performances on a readingwriting test. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 95 , 426433.Hazlett, E. A.,Buchsbaum, M. S., Hsieh, P.,Haznedar, M. M.,Platholi, J., LiCalzi, E. M.,et al. (2004). Regional glucose metabolism withincortical Brodmann areas in

    healthy individuals and autistic patients. Neuropsychobiology, 49, 115125.Haznedar, M. M., Buchsbaum, M. S., Hazlett, E. A., LiCalzi, E. M., Cartwright, C., & Hollander, E. (2006). Volumetric analysis and three-dimensional glucose

    metabolic mapping of the striatum and thalamus in patients with autism spectrum disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(7), 12521263.

    Haznedar, M.M., Buchsbaum,M. S.,Metzger, M.,Solimando, A.,Spiegel-Cohen,J., & Hollander,E. (1997). Anterior cingulate gyrus volume andglucose metabolismin autistic disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 10471050.

    Haznedar, M. M., Buchsbaum, M. S., Wei, T. C., Hof, P. R., Cartwright, C., Bienstock, C. A., et al. (2000). Limbic circuitry in patients with autism spectrum disordersstudied with positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 19942001.

    Henderson, H.,Schwartz, C.,Mundy,P., Burnette, C.,Sutton, S., Zahka, N.,et al.(2006). Response monitoring, the error-related negativity, and differences in socialbehavior in autism. Brain and Cognition, 61(1), 96109.

    Hester, R., Fassbender, C., & Garavan, H. (2004). Individual differences in error processing: A review and reanalysis of three event-related fMRI studies using Go/Nogo task. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 986994.

    Hill, E. L., & Russell, J. (2002). Action memory and self-monitoring in children with autism: Self versus other. Infant and Child Development, 11(2), 159170.Hong Kong Education Department & Hong Kong Psychological Society. (1981). The Hong Kong Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children. Hong Kong: Government

    Printer (in Chinese).Hughes, C., & Russell, J. (1993). Autistic childrens difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: Its implications for theories of autism. Developmental

    Psychology, 29, 498510.Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32, 477492.Ishii,R., Shinosaki,K., Ukai, S.,Inouye, T.,Ishihara,T., Yoshimine, T.,et al.(1999). Medial prefrontalcortexgeneratesfrontal midlinethetarhythm. NeuroReport,10 ,

    675679.John, E. R., Prichep, L., Ahn, H., Easton, P., Fridman, J., & Kaye, H. (1983). Neurometric evaluation of cognitive dysfunctions and neurological disorders in children.

    Progress in Neurobiology, 21, 239290.John, E. R., Prichep, L. S., Fridman, J., & Easton, P. (1988). Neurometrics: Computer-assisted differential diagnosis of brain dysfunctions. Science, 239, 162169.

    A.S. Chan et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (2011) 254266264

  • 8/3/2019 Hong Kong Autism

    12/13

    Johnson, C. P., Myers, S. M., & Council on Children with Disabilities, (2007). Identification and evaluation of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics,120, 11831215.

    Kana, R.K., Keller,T. A.,Minshew,N. J.,& Just,M. A. (2007).Inhibitory control in high-functioning autism:Decreasedactivation andunderconnectivity ininhibitionnetworks. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 198206.

    Kleinhans, N., Akshoomoff, N., & Delis, D. C. (2005). Executive functions in autism and Aspergers disorder: Flexibility, fluency, and inhibition. DevelopmentalNeuropsychology, 27, 379401.

    Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews, 29, 169195.Kok, A., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Ullsperger, M. (2006). The control of attention and actions: Current research and future developments. Brain Research, 1105, 16.Kropotov, J. D., Crawford, H. J., & Polyakov, Y. I. (1997). Somatosensory event-related potential changes to painful stimuli during hypnotic analgesia: Anterior

    cingulate cortex and anterior temporal cortex intracranial recordings. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 27, 18.

    Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 11151122.Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Liddle, P. F., Kiehl, K. A., & Smith, A. M. (2001). Event-related fMRI study of response inhibition. Human Brain Mapping, 12, 100109.Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2002). Autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Luna, B., Minshew, N. J., Garver, K. E., Lazar, N. A., Thulborn, K. R., Eddy, W. F., et al. (2002). Neocortical system abnormalities in autism. Neurology, 59, 834840.Mahone, E. M., Koth, C. W., Cutting, L., Singer, H. S., & Denckla, M. B. (2001). Executive function in fluency and recall measures among children with Tourette

    syndrome or ADHD. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 102111.Menon, V., Adleman, N. E., White, C. D., Glover, G. H., & Reiss, A. L. (2001). Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task. Human Brain

    Mapping, 12(3), 131143.Miller, J. O., & Low, K. (2001). Motor processes in simple, go/no-go, and choice reaction time tasks: A psychophysiological analysis. Journal of Experimental

    Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 266289.Minshew, N. J, Meyer, J., & Goldstein, G. (2002). Abstract reasoning in autism: A dissociation between concept formation and concept identification.

    Neuropsychology, 16, 327334.Monastra, V. J., Lubar, J. F., & Linden, M. (2001). The development of a quantitative electroencephalographic scanning process for attention deficit-hyperactivity

    disorder: Reliability and validity studies. Neuropsychology, 15, 136144.Monastra, V. J., Lubar, J. F., Linden, M., VanDeusen, P., Green, G., Wing, W., et al. (1999). Assessing attention deficit hyperactivity disorder via quantitative

    electroencephalography: An initial validation study. Neuropsychology, 13, 424433.

    Mundy,P. (2003). Annotation: Theneuralbasisof socialimpairments in autism: Therole of thedorsalmedial-frontalcortex andanterior cingulate system.Journalof Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 793809.

    Murias, M., Webb, S. J., Greenson, J., & Dawson, G. (2007). Resting state cortical connectivity reflected in EEG coherence in individuals with autism. BiologicalPsychiatry, 62, 270273.

    Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibitiontaxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 220246.

    Nyden, A.,Gillberg,C., Hjelmquist, E.,& Heiman,M. (1999).Executive function/attention deficits in boys with Asperger syndrome, attentiondisorder andreading/writing disorder. Autism, 3, 213228.

    Onton, J., Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2005). Frontal midline EEG dynamics during working memory. Neuroimage, 27, 341356.Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental

    Disorders, 29(2), 171177.Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (1997). Inhibitory function in nonretarded children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 5977.Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Lehmann, D., Koenig, T., Kochi, K., Merlo, M. C. G., Hell, D., et al. (1999). Low resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA)

    functional imaging in acute, neuroleptic-nave, first-episode, productive schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 90, 169179.

    Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Michel, C. M., & Lehmann, D. (1994). Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: A new method for localizing electrical activity in thebrain. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 4965.

    Pinkerton, F., Watson, D. R., & McClelland, R. J. (1989). A neurophysiological study of children with reading, writing and spelling difficulties. Developmental

    Medicine and Child Neurology, 31, 569581.Pizzagalli, D. A.,Oakes, T. R.,& Davidson,R. J. (2003). Coupling of theta activity andglucose metabolism in thehuman rostralanterior cingulate cortex:An EEG/PET

    study of normal and depressed subjects. Psychophysiology, 40, 939949.

    Pizzagalli, D.,Pascual-Marqui, R. D.,Nitschke, J. B.,Oakes, T. R.,Larson, C. L., Abercrombie, H. C.,et al. (2001). Anterior cingulate activity as a predictor of degree oftreatment response in major depression: Evidence from brain electrical tomography analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 405415.

    Posner, M. I., & DiGirolamo, G. J. (1998). Executive attention: Conflict, target detection, and cognitive control. In R. Parasuraman (Ed.), The attentive brain.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 2542.Reischies, F. M., Neuhaus, A. H., Hansen, M. L., Mientus, S., Mulert, C., & Gallinat, J. (2005). Electrophysiological and neuropsychological analysis of delirious state:

    The role of the anterior cingulate gyrus. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 138, 171181.Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). Oops! Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured

    and normal subjects Neuropsychologia, 35, 747758.Rossion, B.,& Pourtois,G. (2004). Revisiting Snodgrassand Vanderwarts object pictorialset: Therole of surface detail in basic-level object recognition. Perception,

    33, 217236.Russell, J., & Jarrold, C. (1998). Error-correction problems in autism: Evidence for a monitoring impairment? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28 ,

    177188.Russell, J., & Jarrold, C. (1999). Memory for actions in children with autism: Self versus other. Journal of Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 4(4), 303331.

    Russell, J., Jarrold, C., & Hood, B. (1999). Two intact executive capacities in children with autism: Implications for the core executive dysfunctions in the disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2), 103112.

    Sauseng, P., Hoppe, J., Klimesch, W., Gerloff, C., & Hummel, F. C. (2007). Dissociation of sustained attention from central executive functions: Local activity andinter-regional connectivity in the theta range. European Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 587593.

    Schmid, R. G., Tirsch, W. S., & Reitmeir, P. (1997). Correlation of developmental neurological findings with spectral analytical EEG evaluations in pre-school agechildren. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 103, 516527.

    Schmitz, N.,Rubia,K., Daly, E.,Smith,A., Williams, S.,& Murphy,D. G. M. (2006). Neural correlates of executive functionsin autistic spectrum disorders. BiologicalPsychiatry, 59, 716.

    Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, B. R. (1998). The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Shafritz, K. M., Dichter, G. S., Baranek, G. T., & Belger, A. (2008). The neural circuitry mediating shifts in behavioral response and cognitive set in autism. Biological

    Psychiatry, 63(10), 974980.Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity.

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174215.Somsen, R. J. M, Van-Klooster, B. J., Van-der-Molen, M. W., & Van-Leeuwen, H. M. (1997). Growth spurts in brain maturation during middle childhood as indexed

    by EEG power spectra. Biological Psychology, 44, 187209.Sturm, H., Fernell, E., & Gillberg, C. (2004). Autism spectrum disorders in children with normal intellectual levels: Associated impairments and subgroups.

    Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 46, 444447.

    Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Palumbo, C. L., Buckle, L., Sayer, L., & Pogue, J. (1994). Organizational strategies of patients with unilateral or bilateral frontal lobeinjury in word list learning tasks. Neuropsychology, 8, 355373.

    A.S. Chan et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (2011) 254266 265

  • 8/3/2019 Hong Kong Autism

    13/13

    Stuss, D. T., Binns, M. A., Murphy, K. J., & Alexander, M. P. (2002). Dissociations within the anterior attentional system: Effects of task complexity and irrelevantinformation on reaction time speed and accuracy. Neuropsychology, 16, 500513.

    Taylor, S. F., Stern, E. R., & Gehring, W. J. (2007). Neural systems for error monitoring: Recent findings and theoretical perspectives. Neuroscientist, 13, 160172.Thakkar, K. N., Polli, F. E., Joseph, R. M., Tuch, D. S., Hadjikhani, N., Barton, J. J. S., et al. (2008). Response monitoring, repetitive behaviour and anterior cingulate

    abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Brain, 131, 24642478.VanVeen, V.,Cohen, J. D.,Botvinick, M. M.,Stenger, V. A.,& Carter,C. S. (2001).Anteriorcingulate cortex,conflict monitoring, andlevelsof processing. Neuroimage,

    14, 13021308.Wing, L. (1997). The autistic spectrum. Lancet, 350, 17611766.

    A.S. Chan et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (2011) 254266266