33436886

Upload: bently-johnson

Post on 03-Apr-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    1/24

    O R I G I N A L P A P E R

    American Republican Religion? Disentangling the

    Causal Link Between Religion and Politics in the US

    Stratos Patrikios

    Published online: 5 February 2008 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008

    Abstract Recent research in American political behavior has examined at length

    the link between evangelical Protestants and the Republican Party. These works

    however do not consider the idiosyncratic nature of religiosity in the US, and insist

    on treating religion as an unmoved mover with respect to political contexts. The

    question posed herein is: during the participation of religious communities in par-

    tisan politics, should we expect politics to eventually constrain religious behavior?

    Motivated by a political social identity approach, I use American National ElectionStudy panel data and structural equation modeling techniques to explore the

    untested possibility that religious and political factors are linked through reciprocal

    causation. Conditional upon religious and temporal context, findings highlight the

    causal impact of ideology and partisanship in shaping religious behavior.

    Keywords Religious politicization Church attendance Party identification Ideology Social identity theory

    Introduction

    The nexus between religion and politics in violent and non-violent conflicts tends to

    generate global scholarly and popular attention. American society in particular

    serves as a proverbial case, where the peaceful but vocal participation of religious

    With apologies to Alexis de Tocqueville.

    Electronic supplementary material Theonline version of this article (doi:10.1007/s11109-008-9053-1)

    contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

    Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389

    DOI 10.1007/s11109-008-9053-1

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11109-008-9053-1http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11109-008-9053-1
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    2/24

    populations in the political process is now widely considered an endemic

    phenomenon (Leege and Kellstedt 1993; Green et al. 2003). The relationship has

    been identified as a cleavage, a concept describing the translation of objective social

    divisions into enduring political conflicts, with original reference to the formation of

    West European party systems (Lipset and Rokkan 1967).Given the interest and concerned tone of most secular social science on the

    matter, it is surprising that research has tended to probe the role of religion in

    politics relative only to the impact of stable religious variables on political

    behavior. This sociological interpretation of politics expects that exogenous

    religious processes (e.g. exposure to church contexts) shape political behavior

    (e.g. partisanship), and explains relevant trends accordingly. Recent studies in this

    direction have debated the drift of mainline Protestants away from the Republican

    Party, the stability of Catholic traditional support for the Democrats, but above all

    the entrance of evangelical Protestants into the Republican electoral base in the1980s and their transformation into an efficient political machine in the 1990s, as

    described in Fig. 1 (e.g. Moen 1994; Wilcox 1996; Guth et al. 1997; Layman 1997,

    2001; Manza and Brooks 1999; Bolzendahl and Brooks 2005).

    The present study examines the relationship between evangelical Protestantism

    and the Republican Party in recent decades. However, it avoids the aforementioned

    sociological reductionism and its limited focus on the political impact of religious

    factors, such as churches, para-church organizations, lobbies and demographics.

    Instead the following discussion shifts to the effects of politicization on religious

    behavior itself, defined here as church attendance. In this way, I pursue a morenuanced direction in the study of cleavages, and propose that the products of the

    infusion of religion into politics are not restricted to the electoral realm, but can

    potentially transform religion into a secular/political phenomenon. According to this

    path, the influence of religious practice on political behaviora conventional

    368 Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    3/24

    assumption adopted by most political scientistscan be supplemented by a reverse

    effect, whereby religious practice becomes constrained by political/partisan

    concerns (cf. Sartori 1969; Kriesi 1998).

    Michael Hout and Claude Fischers sociological study (2002) is to my knowledge

    the only published quantitative effort that attempts to explore this expectation, as aninvestigation of political pressures on individual religiosity in the context of the

    recent religious politicization in the US. The investigation centers on apostasy, i.e.

    the phenomenon of Christians dropping out of church. The authors argue that the

    conservative religious politicization of the 1990s caused the following backlash:

    ideologically liberal and moderate Christians abandoning conservative denomina-

    tions. Hout and Fischer interpret part of this movement as a reaction against the

    Christian Rights political agenda, and the prominent place occupied by conser-

    vative Protestantism in GOP ranks (2002, pp. 181, 185).

    The present article builds on Hout and Fischers effort, updating it in three ways.First, the authors provide only an indirect test of their expectation using cross-

    sectional data; the models presented herein avoid assumptions of temporal

    precedence by turning to panel data, and provide therefore a more rigorous test

    of the hypothesis. Second, the 2002 work focuses on how personal ideological

    orientation determines apostasy. In what follows, I elaborate on this idea and

    develop an additional explanation rooted in partisan influences, on the basis of a

    well-documented phenomenon in realignment research. This refers to the sorting-

    out, i.e. the overlap between ideology and partisanship, and the polarization

    experienced between the two major parties since the late 1970s. In thisdevelopment, culturally liberal Republicans abandoned the GOP and conservative

    Democrats followed a similar movement away from their party (e.g. Poole and

    Rosenthal 1984; Abramowitz and Saunders 1998; Levendusky 2005). Finally, my

    work focuses on changes in religious practice and not on apostasy, since the low

    number of apostates in the datasets is inadequate for multivariate analysis, and

    relevant repeated measures are missing.

    My expectation is that the politically charged American religious landscape, in

    particular the close association in the public mind of evangelical Protestants with

    social conservatives and the Republican Party, can lead individuals to react by

    altering their religious behavior. Religious politicization, loosely defined as the

    perceived affinity of certain religious populations with certain parties or ideological

    camps, creates the potential to make some believers minimize their attendance at

    certain churches and others to increase their church-going. This process is

    summarized as an extrinsically political religion fuelled by ideology and

    partisanship. The two competing expectations are specified as: (i) an attendance

    effect on politics (the sociological approach) and (ii) an ideological/partisan effect

    on attendance (the political religion phenomenon).

    Based on social identity theory with a strong emphasis on partisanship (Greene

    1999, 2002, 2004; cf. Green et al. 2002), I use panel data from the Michigan/

    American National Election Study (ANES) pool, which both precede and overlap

    Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389 369

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    4/24

    cross-lagged effects models tests competing expectations. Results support the

    political religion hypothesis, either ideological or partisan, and open the way for a

    novel understanding of the continuing impact of the political process even on non-

    political variables, and of the worldly nature of religious behavior.

    Due to the absence of relevant data on religious affiliation and identification, theanalysis is based on the interplay of political factors (ideology and partisanship)

    with church attendance. The argument begins with the American religious

    exceptionalism thesis, and continues by reviewing a recent attempt to revitalize

    the party identification concept as social identification. The former discussion

    supports the use of religious variables as endogenous to the political process, while

    the latter allows for the possibility of partisan and ideological effects on religious

    variables. The analysis then continues with a consideration of methodological

    problems, followed by an interpretation of findings and their implications for

    research on electoral politics and religion.

    American Religion in the Left-Hand Side

    Based on an assumed equivalence, the use of religious explanations in American

    political research often reflects an idealized European experience of religion as a

    stable, politically exogenous phenomenon. These features are treated as adequate

    reasons for the use of religious variables as stable demographics. Sociologists of

    religion however have long agreed on the idiosyncratic nature of American piety,especially among native Protestants. First, the civil religion phenomenon (Bellah

    1967) challenges the idea that Christian faith in the US merely reflects theological

    concerns. Civil or general religion represents faith in the American way of life and

    the status of the US as Gods chosen nation. This infusion of religion with secular

    values provides a first step for recognizing the partly secular component in the

    religiosity of American Christians.

    Second, the religious market metaphor claims that the existence of a plethora of

    churches in America (religious pluralism) creates the conditions that reinforce

    religiosity among the American population (Finke and Stark 1992). This deregu-

    lated setting implies that churches have to tailor/improve their products and even

    offer less spiritual benefits, in order to increase demand. Again, it appears that the

    religious package put forward by American churches, especially the less centralized

    Protestant ones, has a demystified quality, one that is infused with secular benefits.

    Finally, the character of the products offered by American religious institutions

    goes hand in glove with the motives behind the preferences of American believers. In

    a pluralistic religious market located within a heavily consumerist society, it is

    reasonable to suggest that the public experience of religiosity is closer to that of

    voluntary association, and not of passive socialization (Newport 1979). The idea of

    religion as an active, conscious choice sounds less eccentric when we consider one

    phenomenon with prominence in the relevant literature: denominational switching

    370 Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    5/24

    This type of mobility, practiced by up to a third of Americans at some point in their

    life, supports an image of religion as a self-selective, dynamic choice that cannot be

    treated as permanent (Warner 1993; Loveland 2003). Overall, the American religious

    exceptionalism thesis challenges the use of religious factors as stable demographics,

    and instead maintains that religiosity is a volatile choice, open to secular influences.This study uses this understanding and posits that in specific circumstances, part of

    these influences can take the form of ideological and partisan concerns.

    A Political Explanation of American Religiosity

    The theoretical justification of my argument is based on a social identity

    conceptualization of party identification, which can also be extended to identifi-

    cation with ideological camps. I argue that this provides an explanation of howpoliticizationeither on ideological or partisan linesinitiates a transformation of

    religion. The Michigan school and its disciples have treated identification with a

    party as a multifaceted concept: evaluation of partisan objects, perceptual screen for

    interpreting incoming stimuli, and psychological expression of group membership

    (e.g. Campbell et al. 1960; Miller and Shanks 1996). The concept has been

    subsequently challenged as endogenous to policy preferences, ideological predis-

    positions, evaluations of candidates and government performance (e.g. Nie et al.

    1979; Page and Jones 1979; Fiorina 1981).

    Recent theoretical and methodological advances however, have reinstated thetraditional view of party identification as a long-term predisposition, which affects

    voters evaluations and perceptions of political reality and even their core political

    predispositions, without being affected by them in the short term (Green and

    Palmquist 1990; Green et al. 2002; Goren 2005). Miller and Shanks summarize this

    view: one of the roles of the church, or the party, is to provide structure to the

    ordinary persons understanding of the external world[and] cues for normative

    assessments of the outside world (1996, p. 121).

    Instead of being treated as a simple attitude towards a political object, party

    identification can be conceptualized through social identity theory (SIT) as

    psychological belonging to a social group (for an overview of theoretical and

    measurement issues see Greene 2002, 2004). In general, the group psychological

    effects identified by SIT are termed social identification and self-categorization.

    These correspond to a definition of the self according to group characteristics, and

    to an exaggeration of differences between own group and other groups in order to

    achieve a positive self-concept, respectively (Tajfel 1981; Long and Spears 1997).

    For SIT, an individual that feels closer to a group tends to internalize group

    membership by seeing herself through group stereotypes and not through personal

    characteristicsI am Democrat/Catholic/Hispanic/Liberal.

    The cognitive process of self-categorization explains how SIT works, that is,

    through intergroup social classification (Turner 1985). People assign social objects

    Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389 371

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    6/24

    distinctive than the out-group by conducting biased comparisons (stereotypes) with

    members of the out-group.

    According to social identity and self-categorization theory then, individuals will

    (a) tend to perceive themselves less as unique units and more as group members; (b)

    cement their impulse for a positive self-image on exaggerated comparisons without-group members; and (c) follow in-group standards in attitude and produce

    groupy behavior (Hogg and Terry 2000, p. 121). The three points are interrelated

    in the sense that self-perceived membership to the same group expects conformity

    with shared in-group standards (and against out-group standards) in order to achieve

    greatest possible perceived inter-group distinctiveness.

    The original exposition of the party identification concept in The American Voter

    already contained an emphasis on this belonging dimension of partisanship, in the

    sense of an us versus them distinction, besides its function as a perceptual

    screen or political attitude (Campbell et al. 1960, pp. 1335). The SIT approachtowards partisanship then expects that citizens who experience greater group

    identification with fellow partisans (gauged with scales that measure feelings of

    belonging to a social group) will tend to internalize in-party and out-party

    stereotypes, exhibit increased engagement in partisan behavior, like rally atten-

    dance, and exaggerate differences between us and them, even after controlling

    for the effect of the traditional party identification variable (Greene 2004).

    The above opens the way for examining the overlooked effect of party

    identification on individual religious characteristics. Typically, parties raise the

    salience of social-political links by sending out clear references via electionmanifestos, candidate speeches and policy proposals. Famous among many examples

    is the 1992 Republican National Convention, especially the culture war speech by

    Pat Buchanan, which represents a symbolic milestone in the partys effort to associate

    itself with conservative family values, popular among theologically conservative

    populations. This effort has not been limited to the 1990s, as evident by an inspection

    of the news since 2000, i.e. since George W. Bushs election in the White House. The

    President, a born-again Christian, has consistently stressed the link between

    Republicanism and theologically conservative religion. For instance, in what appears

    to be a conservative reading of Christian faith he hindered federal funding of pro-

    choice groups abroad in 2001, while taking steps to promote funding for religious

    service organizations. Bush has also openly declared his opposition to same-sex

    marriage by favoring a constitutional amendment that made it illegal in 2004. It is not

    surprising then that evangelical leader Jerry Falwell described Bushs reelection in

    2004 and the role played by evangelicals in it as: a slam dunk as the Church of

    Jesus Christ made the difference in initiating the return of this nation to moral sanity

    and the Judeo-Christian ethic (quoted in Layman and Hussey 2005, p. 1).

    The outcomes of such recurring cliches that directly or indirectly identify two

    labels as naturally connected (Republicanevangelical Protestant or conservative-

    evangelical Protestant) can be twofold. If we focus on the religious group as our

    building block, as political science normally does opting for a sociological

    372 Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    7/24

    closer to the GOP, which constitutes only one interpretation of the trend described

    in Fig. 1.

    Yet, members of religious groups are also attached to partisan groups, identities

    that become particularly energized during election periods. If we concentrate on the

    partisan in-group, self-categorization theory expects that labels associated with thein-party will guide its members towards adopting similar meanings of reality and

    desirable preferences, or actions. If in-group (party) members are Republicans, and

    assuming that the link between social group and party is very prominent, members

    will be exposed to the stereotype (the social imagery of the party), which connects, for

    instance, Republicanism with evangelical Protestantism, and Democrats with

    Catholicism, or in more recent years, with secularism. In this case, religiosity

    becomes an in-group (party) norm and according to SIT, group identifiers will be

    more likely to follow this religious norm. In a self-selective process, Republicans will

    tend to stress their Republicanism by intensifying their commitment to Evangelicalchurches; alternatively, Democrats will be under pressure to distance themselves

    from this religious environment, which contradicts their partisan in-group norms.1

    A similar rationale could apply to ideological camps (Lau 1989; Deaux et al.

    1995). According to Hout and Fischer (2002), the conservative flavor dominating

    evangelical churches since the 1970s has driven non-conservative evangelicals

    away from those churches. Using SIT, the distancing of moderate/liberal believers

    from evangelical churches can be interpreted as the result of groupy behavior.

    Here, liberal evangelicals would be expected to follow in-group (liberal) standards

    and react to the conservative turn of their churches by avoiding religious services.Conversely, conservative evangelicals would tend to adopt in-group (conservative)

    standards and strengthen their links with evangelical churches by attending more

    frequently. In politicized periods, the connection of evangelicalism with political

    conservatism would have attendance become the norm of the ideological

    (conservative) in-group, with the reverse holding for the opposite (liberal) in-

    group. This process should accompany the conventionally assumed influence of

    church on ideological orientation.

    Measurement and Analytic Strategy

    The starting point in my effort to test the hypothesized bidirectional causal link

    between religiosity and political variables is a specific field in the party identification

    scholarship, which explores the possibility of reciprocal causation between

    partisanship and other political variables (Jackson 1975; Markus and Converse

    1979; Page and Jones 1979; Layman and Carsey 2002; Goren 2005; Carsey and

    Layman 2006). This motivation, coupled with the theoretical emphasis of SIT on

    partisan groups, justifies the preference given by the following methodological

    discussion to partisanship over ideology. The same analytic framework however isused for assessing the effect of ideology on church attendance.

    Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389 373

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    8/24

    To evaluate the religious influence on politics and the parallel political influence

    on religious behavior, I use ANES panel data. Three electoral cycles are covered:

    19721976, 19921996, and 20002004.2 Following common practice (e.g. Lenski

    1963) African Americans have been excluded from the analysis. This segment

    appears only in low numbers in ANES samples, and also is distinctive in terms ofhistorical and demographic characteristics, and organizational autonomy within the

    Protestant family. The variable that operationalizes social identification with a party

    is the ANES partisanship item: a seven-point scale, with high scores showing

    Republicanism. Although not a perfect measure of psychological group identifica-

    tion, the Michigan item could be considered an adequate indicator of such processes

    when no alternative is available (Greene 2002). The ideological self-placement

    scale is selected to tap identification with the liberal and conservative camps; high

    scores on the seven-point scale represent conservatism. I use the original root

    question, and not the summary three-point measure, which also allocates leaners.This ensures that when examining reciprocity between ideology and attendance,

    both scales have a high number of points. Due to limited item availability in the

    panels, the dimension of religion examined is church attendance, a five-point scale

    with high scores indicating frequent attendance.3 Table 1 contains summary

    statistics for intra-group trends across waves.

    With the exception of Hout and Fischer (2002), almost all studies of the

    relationship between religion and politics, in the American case and beyond,

    propose unidirectional causal effects from religious variables to political ones (for

    instance, see contributions in Leege and Kellstedt 1993). In assuming the soleexistence of this flow of causality, these studies are content to model cross-sectional

    data, drawn at a single point in time. Considering the typical conjecture in such

    research, namely that religious variables represent fixed personal characteristics,

    temporally prior and exogenous to the political process, analyses without a temporal

    dimension seem to serve the purpose. Yet, the untested assumption of unidirectional

    causation can easily be evaluated when temporal precedence is embedded in the

    data, i.e. when repeated measurement of the same individuals is available across

    time (Finkel 1995, pp. 223).4

    2 Data used in the present study were made available by the Inter-University Consortium for Political and

    Social Research. The author holds sole responsibility for their analysis and interpretation. Results from

    supplementary analyses mentioned in the text are available from the author. Throughout the study, I avoid

    using the 19561960 ANES panel, since the detailed measure that differentiates among Protestant

    denominations and is required for sub-group analysis had not yet been introduced in 1956. Also, in the

    examination of the reciprocal link between ideology and attendance, the 20002004 panel is dropped,

    since there is no repeated measure of ideological self-placement in 2004.3 Note that the item may be subject to misreporting due to social desirability effects, a problem however

    that cannot be corrected with ANES data (e.g. Hadaway et al. 1993).4 Establishment of causal precedence is possible with cross-sectional data, when simultaneous effects are

    assumed between two variables (Finkel 1995). This however requires the use of unrealistic modeling

    374 Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    9/24

    Still, most investigations remain innocent as to what happens to religiouscharacteristics once they are exposed to partisan politics. The causal relationship

    proposed in this analysis takes this step and identifies two mutually reinforcing

    effects: a religious effect on politics (the orthodox assumption in political science),

    and a political effect on religion. Their combination constitutes a feedback

    phenomenon. The causal link between religiosity and partisanship is specified as a

    cross-lagged effects model with the following structure (see Fig. 2):

    PartyIDt b1PartyIDt1 b2Attendancet1 e1t 1

    Attendancet b3Attendancet1 b4PartyIDt1 e2t 2

    Table 1 Descriptive statistics for 19721976, 19921996, 20002004 panels: means (standard

    deviations)

    Variable [scale] Catholic Mainline Evangelical

    Descriptive statistics for 19721976 panela

    Party ID 1972 [06] 2.18 (1.90) 3.37 (2.02) 2.84 (1.98)

    Party ID 1976 [06] 2.11 (1.88) 3.29 (1.98) 2.66 (1.96)

    Ideology 1972 [17] 4.18 (1.16) 4.30 (1.19) 4.62 (1.03)

    Ideology 1976 [17] 4.25 (1.19) 4.51 (1.23) 4.81 (1.14)

    Attendance 1972 [15] 3.73 (1.49) 3.00 (1.41) 3.41 (1.49)

    Attendance 1976 [15] 3.63 (1.48) 2.94 (1.40) 3.50 (1.48)

    Descriptive statistics for 19921996 panelb

    Party ID 1992 [06] 2.54 (1.80) 3.97 (1.73) 3.28 (1.98)

    Party ID 1996 [06] 2.59 (2.05) 4.11 (1.95) 3.49 (2.02)Ideology 1992 [17] 4.16 (1.35) 4.58 (1.30) 4.59 (1.53)

    Ideology 1996 [17] 4.18 (1.35) 4.79 (1.21) 4.76 (1.33)

    Attendance 1992 [15] 3.37 (1.56) 2.71 (1.51) 3.33 (1.59)

    Attendance 1996 [15] 3.31 (1.55) 2.72 (1.44) 3.16 (1.59)

    Descriptive statistics for 20002004 panelc

    Party ID 2000 [06] 3.13 (2.06) 3.22 (2.11) 3.35 (1.97)

    Party ID 2004 [06] 3.30 (2.24) 3.35 (2.25) 3.63 (2.16)

    Attendance 2000 [15] 3.18 (1.56) 2.91 (1.46) 3.16 (1.63)

    Attendance 2004 [15] 2.89 (1.58) 2.94 (1.51) 3.28 (1.58)a Source: 19721976 ANES panel. Note: Higher scores indicate Republicanism (6 is strong Republican),

    conservatism (7 is extremely conservative) and frequent attendance (5 is every week)b

    Source: 19921996 ANES panel. Note: Higher scores indicate Republicanism, conservatism and fre-

    quent attendancec Source: 20002004 ANES panel. Note: Higher scores indicate Republicanism and frequent attendance.

    No repeated measurement available for ideological self-placement

    Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389 375

    http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    10/24

    influence of attendance at time t- 1. An equivalent logic applies to b3 regarding

    church attendance as the dependent variable in Eq. 2. Coefficient b2 is the cross-

    lagged effect of attendance at time t- 1 on partisanship at time t, net of the effect

    of partisan stability. The equation then predicts changes in partisanship. The same

    applies to b4 regarding attendance as the dependent variable. For the effect of

    ideology on attendance, the partisanship variable is replaced by the ideology

    measure.

    The two effects underlying the politicization of religion are: first, that attendance

    shapes partisanship (or ideology); second, that partisanship (or ideology) reinforces

    attendance, i.e. the political religion effect. If the latter is correct, we should not

    only observe b2[0 in Eq. 1, but also b4[ 0 in Eq. 2. Alternatively, if only the

    sociological view that dominates electoral studies is true, we should only observeb2[ 0 in Eq. 1, and b4 = 0 in Eq. 2. As will be argued below in more detail, I

    A. Unidirectional effect

    Political variable

    (t-1)

    Attendance

    (t-1)

    Political variable

    (t)

    Attendance

    (t)

    a

    B. Feedback effect

    Political variable

    (t-1)

    Attendance

    (t-1)

    Political variable(t)

    Attendance

    (t)

    b

    Fig. 2 Model comparison: unidirectional versus feedback effects. Note: The sole causal assumption of

    studies working with cross-sections is represented by coefficient a in panel A. The feedback model adds

    the political effect on attendance represented by coefficient b. Errors, correlations and controls have been

    excluded for simplicity

    376 Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    11/24

    Models were estimated with AMOS 6.0 and full-information-maximum likeli-

    hood (Arbuckle 2005).5 The analysis does not report coefficients from the above

    causal specification (Eqs. 1 and 2) for all subgroups across all panels. An explicit

    test is proposed, which directly compares alternative models. Specifically, I contrast

    the fit of the feedback model (the political-religion phenomenon) to that of aunidirectional model, which postulates that the only effect taking place during

    religious politicization moves from attendance to ideology/partisanship (see Fig. 2).

    This constitutes a more explicit evaluation of the unidirectional hypothesis versus

    the reciprocal one (Bollen 1989, pp. 2912).

    The comparison is conducted through a chi-square difference test, since the

    constrained model with unidirectional effects is nested within the unconstrained

    model that proposes the feedback. The difference between the chi-square values of

    the two nested models is itself distributed as a chi-square value with degrees of

    freedom equal to the constraints imposed on the second model. For reasons ofpresentational parsimony, I only present results when the reciprocal hypothesis

    (unrestricted model, where the ideological/partisan effect on attendance is freely

    estimated) shows a significantly better fit against the traditional, unidirectional

    expectation (restricted model, with the political effect constrained to equal 0). For

    comparison, results that do not support the feedback hypothesis appear in the

    Appendix (Supplementary electronic material).

    The hypothesis of lagged instead of synchronous effects between the main

    variables defines the recursive character of the model, and makes identification

    simple. A specification with synchronous effects would suggest that the two mainfactors influence each other at a single point in time. Such models break the

    condition of independent variables being uncorrelated with the residual, since the

    cause is at the same time influenced by its effect (Finkel 1995, p. 32). In this case

    regression estimates would be biased, therefore analysis must turn to the use of

    instrumental variables (see note 4). Due to heroic assumptions and the gradual

    nature of most social psychological effects, I consider such non-recursive models a

    less plausible scenario. In any case, researchers that employ feedback models of

    ANES data suggest that the selection of lagged over synchronous effects makes no

    difference to the estimation of causal relationships (Goren 2005; Carsey and

    Layman 2006).

    Finally, I have specified additional elements in the cross-effects models, in

    accordance with the literature. First, the error terms in Eqs. 1 and 2 are correlated.

    This allows estimating whether the two dependent variables at time t share at least

    one omitted independent variable (Kline 1998, p. 101). If the disturbances were left

    unrelated, that would represent the less plausible assumption that the two dependent

    variables do not share any common, unobserved, causes. This is especially

    unwarranted, when one considers that partisanship, ideology and religiosity are

    5 FIML does not exclude missing cases from analysis for respondents included in the models. Under the

    Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389 377

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    12/24

    outcomes of the same pre-adult socialization process. Standard demographic

    controls were added as predictors of ideology/partisanship and religiosity at time t,

    together with their inter-correlations (see notes in Tables 3 and 5).

    The Impact of Context

    A central element in SIT is that social identities are dynamic, i.e. their strength and

    influence depends on temporal and social context (Hogg and Terry 2000). Huddys

    critique of SIT (2003) emphasizes such situational influences, which result in the

    variable salience of identities relative to settings. In essence, context can result in

    individuals switching from one social identity to another. Consider the example of a

    politician who openly stresses differences between two ethnic groups during her

    campaign. Here, identification with ethnic groups is likely to become the dominantidentity among the public. In another instance, the participation of an in-group

    member as a candidate in elections could also raise the visibility of the in-group and

    out-group demarcation (Huddy 2003, pp. 533, 543). The point of context-dependent

    identity salience guides the selection of multiple-group estimation, discrete time-

    lags, and emphasizes the importance of presidential election campaigns in

    highlighting the religion-politics nexus. In the following, I justify these three

    features of the models.

    First, cross-lagged effects were estimated separately for each major religious

    subculture in the US. The classification of churches into three major religiousgroups (Catholic, mainline and evangelical Protestant) follows a standard catego-

    rization scheme, which allocates denominations into broader religious families (see

    Steensland et al. 2000).6 Contextual differences among the three groups make this

    decision essential (cf. Layman and Green 2005). For instance, the lack of vocal

    electoral participation within Roman Catholicism is one element distinguishing this

    religious tradition in the US, and making the expectation of substantial effects

    produced by the linking of religion with politics less plausible.

    Regarding analytic discrimination between mainline and evangelical Protestant

    denominations, a main element of my hypothesis is that politicization takes place to

    different extents and in different periods for the two groups. Research generally agrees

    that the initially higher socioeconomic standing of mainline Protestants has led to a

    different timing of the political mobilization phenomenon compared with evangel-

    icals, who entered the political arena at a later phase (Roof and McKinney 1987; Finke

    and Stark1992; Manza and Brooks 1999). This justifies a separate estimation of the

    hypothesized relationships for mainline and evangelical Protestants.

    6

    Steensland et al. classify nondenominational Protestants as evangelicals according to their churchattendance (frequent attendance suggests evangelicalism) (2000, p. 316). Since the present analysis is

    built on the church attendance variable, adopting the above practice would have introduced a biased logic:

    378 Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    13/24

    The phenomenon of religious behavior driven by partisan (or ideological)

    concerns is hypothesized to emerge within a religious community where ideological

    or partisan identities are increasingly salient. I locate this condition in the post-

    1980s political era among evangelicals, who were at the forefront of electoral

    politics during two periods covered by the data.7

    Specifically, the mid-1990s were atime of extreme cultural polarization and salient religious and political identities.

    Similarly, the early 2000s have been a period of equally prominent connections

    between the GOP and conservative Protestantism, and witnessed a reborn Christian

    heading the Republican ticket in an atmosphere of cultural polarization. It remains

    an empirical question however whether the feedback hypothesis holds among

    evangelicals for the period covered by the 19721976 data. This was a time when

    evangelical Protestants were already ideologically energized by the Social Issue,

    but not yet explicitly attracted by the GOP. Hence, the link between Republicanism

    and evangelical Protestantism had not yet been consolidated in the public mind.After all, 1976, the Year of the Evangelical, was defined by the Democratic

    candidacy of Jimmy Carter.

    The stratification by religious tradition unfortunately encounters practical

    obstacles, which render it susceptible to a degree of criticism. ANES panels

    normally measure denominational affiliation only in the first wave, under the

    obvious assumption that switching does not take placeor that the phenomenon is

    irrelevant to politics. For instance in the 20002004 panel (repeated measures of

    denominational affiliation not available), missing repeated measurement of

    affiliation requires definition of religious groups as follows: individuals areassigned to the Catholic group, for example, if they are members of this group in the

    first wave of the panel. The obvious assumption is that these individuals remain

    members of the same religious community in subsequent waves. In other words,

    division of the sample into religious traditions assumes stability in religious

    preference across waves, an expectation challenged by the switching phenomenon.

    It would have been more reassuring to define each religious group by selecting

    respondents that consistently belonged to the same church across panel waves, but

    this was not feasible especially for the 20002004 study. In this sense, it is a

    possibility that some participants defined as Catholics or evangelicals in the

    first wave may have already dropped out of religion by the following wave or

    converted.

    Second, the time distance between measurements is crucial to the detection of

    causal effects. The problem of specifying the most plausible, if any, delay between

    cause and effect does not have a straightforward solution (Asher 1983; Bollen

    1989). It depends partly on the theoretical conception of the causal effects and partly

    on data restrictions. The use of ANES three-wave panel data constrains my selection

    of time-intervals between cause and effect, since the first and final waves are

    7 The effects reported in this paper hold even with more homogenous stratifications. For instance, if

    models are estimated within born-again evangelicals (measure not available in 19721976), the partisan

    Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389 379

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    14/24

    administered close to presidential elections, while the middle one is collected in a

    mid-term election. One obvious strategy is to specify the effects as continuous

    across time (Finkel 1995, p. 16). It seems plausible to hypothesize that the effect of

    religiosity on political variables takes place evenly distributed across time, and not

    during some arbitrarily defined time intervals, for instance, every two or four years.Yet, this apparently reasonable assumption expects too much on behalf of citizens.

    The idea that voters actively follow political affairs, and continuously update their

    views in meaningful ways loses most of its plausibility against empirical reality

    (Zaller 1992).

    An alternative choice is to model discrete time-lags, with the expectation that

    citizens tend to reassess the link between religion and politics only in specific

    temporal contexts. Election campaigns appear to be ideal as such. In Edelmans

    words elections are rituals and draw attention to common social ties (1964, p. 3).

    During these periods, candidate speeches, advertisements and every day discussionsbring the component parts of the political process to the forefront; voters become

    actively engaged, while ideological and partisan identities obtain increased salience

    (Campbell et al. 1966; Clarke and Stewart 1998). One practical implication is that it

    is reasonable to suggest that the political religion phenomenon is an evaluation

    activated by specific situations during specific periods, and does not take place

    constantly across time.

    Third, the focus on presidential elections is not only necessary for pragmatic

    reasons, in particular the absence of mid-term measurement for church attendance in

    the 19921996 data, but it is also supported for substantive reasons. In justifying theexclusion of the mid-term panel wave from the analysis, I argue that citizens

    re-evaluate the relationship between politics and religion mainly in the abstract

    context of presidential elections and not in the more specific, local context of

    mid-terms. Differences between presidential and congressional electoral settings

    support this decision (e.g. Campbell 1960; Davidson and Oleszek 2004). For the

    above reasons, the same lag (two consecutive presidential elections) was selected for

    all panels. The two-wave specification with observed indicators assumes that

    indicators are perfect measures of the underlying concepts and prohibits correction

    for random measurement error. The addition of a measurement model would

    minimize noise in the variables, but requires either three-wave panel data (for

    single indicators) or multi-item constructs (Finkel 1995). Still, even without a three-

    wave specification and correction for measurement error, the subjectivehence less

    reliableideological and party identification variables exert a non-vanishing

    influence on religious behavior.8

    8 In an earlier stage of the analysis, I used three-wave models with the 19721976 and 20002004

    datasets, which employed a Wiley-Wiley specification with single-indicator latent variables. These

    380 Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    15/24

    Results

    The analysis begins with a test of Hout and Fischers thesis (2002), adjusted to ANES

    panel data. This predicts changes in religious practice due to ideological concerns,

    focusing on the link between political ideology and church attendance. Thepsychological groups of interest identified by SIT are Liberals versus Conservatives.

    Which competing assumptionreciprocity or unidirectionality, i.e. an ideolog-

    ical political religion or the sociological modelbest approximates the causal

    patterns observed in the data? Instead of visually comparing the fit of the feedback

    hypothesis against the fit of the unidirectional hypothesis, Table 2 summarizes

    results from the v2 difference test.9 This test directly compares the feedback

    hypothesis of ideologically driven attendance against the more traditional,

    unidirectional expectation of religious effects on political variables. Highlighted

    entries show which part of the religious population experiences the political religionphenomenon in each period. In both periods, the religious community that practices

    religiosity partly as political behavior is the evangelical group. I consider this a

    product of the entrance of religious populations into ideological camps, especially

    the conservative ideological mobilization of evangelicals by the Social Issue since

    the early 1970s. In this case, members identifying with opposing ideological groups

    tend to adjust their religious exposure accordingly, with liberals avoiding

    ideologically conservative churches and vice versa.

    Table 3 presents in detail the reciprocal models identified above and the

    coefficients connecting ideology and attendance (a seven- and five-point scale,respectively). In the 1970s, it seems that apart from the impact of lagged evangelical

    attendance on changing ideological orientation, we also observe a significant

    political religion effect over and above the influence of control variables and

    lagged attendance. In this case, lagged ideology shapes evangelical attendance, with

    the more conservative tending to increasingly attend evangelical churches and vice

    versa for the more liberal (b = .162). On average, extreme conservatives (seven on

    the ideology scale) increase their attendance by .97 points compared to extreme

    liberals (one on the ideology scale), i.e. by almost one point on the attendance scale.

    We witness a stronger occurrence of the same effect in the 1990s (b = .274),

    whereby extreme conservatives increase their attendance by 1.64 scale points

    compared to extreme liberals.

    The discussion now turns to the posited occurrence of a partisan political religion,

    i.e. the shaping of church attendance by party identification. Table 4 summarizes

    results from the v2 difference test, this time using the partisanship variable in place of

    ideological self-placement. The psychological groups of interest identified by SIT in

    9 Goodness-of-fit is assessed with the following criteria (Arbuckle 2005): the v2 test/degrees of freedom

    ratio, in which values less than 5 are desirable (or 3 for stricter evaluations); Bollens incremental fit

    index (IFI), which makes adjustments for sample size and the complexity of the model, taking intoaccount degrees of freedom, should score close to 90 and above (or .95 and above for more conservative

    evaluations); Bentlers comparative fit index (CFI), which again accounts for small sample sizes and

    Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389 381

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    16/24

    Table 3 (Ideological) Political religion in recent election cycles

    Evangelical (19721976) Evangelical (19921996)

    Stabilities (Time 1 ? Time 2)

    Ideology ? Ideology .245 (.220/.068)***a .571 (.642/.078)***a

    Attendance ? Attendance .608 (.613/.051)*** .707 (.705/.069)***

    Cross-lags (Time 1 ? Time 2)

    Ideology ? Attendance .162 (.113/.076)** .274 (.265/.086)***

    Attendance ? Ideology .092 (.119/.046)** .205 (.239/.062)***

    Summary statistics

    N 244 106

    v2/df 91.201/47 58.047/47

    (1.941, p = .000) (1.235, p = .130)

    IFI, CFI, RMSEA .961, .957, .062 .985, .982, .047

    Sources: 19721976, 19921996 ANES panels

    Note: Maximum likelihood estimates. Parentheses contain standardized coefficients/standard errors.

    Controls included for age, male dummy, education dummies (reference category is high school or less),

    union member in household dummy, marital status dummies (never married is baseline), region dummies

    (northeast is baseline), children in household dummy, and income. Estimates for disturbance correlations,unanalyzed relationships, and control effects are omitted for claritya Ideological stability seems to be artificially low due to the use of the root question; this decision ignores

    Table 2 (Ideological) Political religion: feedback versus unidirectional effects

    Model comparison

    Dv2 Ddf

    19721976 ANES Catholic .138 1

    Mainline 2.577 1

    Evangelical 4.506** 1

    19921996 ANES Catholic .009 1

    Mainline .305 1

    Evangelical 9.639** 1

    Sources: 19721976, 19921996 ANES

    Note: Feedback models with a better fit than unidirectional models are highlighted. The v2 difference

    statistic compares nested models: a feedback model against a constrained unidirectional model, which

    only suggests religious effects on politics. A significant v2 statistic indicates that the constrained model

    (unidirectional effects) is highly unlikely to be valid. In regression analysis terminology, this suggests that

    the effect of lagged ideology on attendance is significant. When the statistic is insignificant, the same

    effect is probably a misspecification. Finally, the 20002004 panel study does not contain a repeated

    measure for ideological self-placement in 2004

    * p\ .10; ** p\ .05

    382 Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389

  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    17/24

    this case are defined as Democrats versus Republicans. Highlighted entries indicate

    which population experienced the political religion phenomenon in each period. Once

    again, attendance seems to be influenced by political (partisan) concerns among

    evangelical Protestants in the 1990s and 2000s, yet the feedback effect is not present

    for this constituency in the 1970s. I attribute this to the absence of partisan polarization

    during that period. That is, the 1970s were still a time when evangelical support for the

    GOP was not yet salient, and the Christian Right had not emerged as a major force in

    American elections. Consistently therefore with the context-dependent nature of SIT

    expectations, evangelicals do not appear to experience partisan pressures on their

    attendance prior to the particization of their churches.10

    Table 4 (Partisan) Political religion: feedback versus unidirectional effects

    Model comparison

    Dv2 Ddf

    19721976 ANES Catholic .171 1

    Mainline 7.608** 1

    Evangelical .118 1

    19921996 ANES Catholic .354 1

    Mainline .575 1

    Evangelical 4.395** 1

    20002004 ANES Catholic .412 1

    Mainline .053 1

    Evangelical 3.719 ** 1

    Sources: 19721976, 19921996, 20002004 ANES panels

    Note: Feedback models with a better fit than unidirectional models are highlighted. The v2 difference

    statistic compares nested models: a feedback model against a constrained unidirectional model, which

    only suggests religious effects on politics. A significant v2 statistic indicates that the constrained model

    (unidirectional effects) is highly unlikely to be valid. In regression analysis terminology, this suggests that

    the effect of lagged partisanship on attendance is significant. When the statistic is insignificant, the same

    effect is probably a misspecification

    * p\ .10; ** p\ .05

    10 According to the test in Table 4, a partisan religion also emerges among mainline Protestants in the

    1970s, whereby partisanship appears to constrain church attendance. One explanation of this could lie with

    the political mobilization of mainline churches during the turbulent 1960s and 1970s, as defined by the Civil

    Rights movement and the Vietnam War (e.g. Wald and Calhoun-Brown 2007). Yet, due to methodological

    problems in model estimation for this subsample in the 1970s, I choose instead to emphasize the absence of

    the political religion effect from the evangelical subsample, which is the focus of the present study.

    Specifically, the cross-effects have different signs for mainline Protestants in the 19721976 model: lagged

    partisanship has a positive effect on changing attendance, while lagged attendance has a counter-intuitive,

    negative effect on changing partisanship (see Supplementary electronic material, Table C). This is a

    suppressor effect, whereby two variables are positively correlated, but direct effects are negative or vice

    versa. Suppression occurs for two main reasons (Smith et al. 1992). First, suppression happens when anadditional predictor is entered in the model, i.e. the true relationship between the variables is in the opposite

    direction than the one indicated by their correlation. If the difference in sign can be explained theoretically,

    Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389 383

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    18/24

    Table 5 presents the actual estimates connecting the main variables within thereciprocal models. Evangelical Protestants go through a round of intense religious

    politicization in the periods covered by the 19921996 and the 20002004 panels.11

    The conventionally assumed impact of lagged evangelical attendance on changing

    partisanship is both substantially and statistically significant in both periods

    (b = .156 and b = .247, respectively). The substantial interpretation of this result is

    that in a period of increasing connections between the GOP and conservative

    Protestantism, frequent exposure to evangelical churches seems to move congre-

    gants closer to the Republican Party. Conversely, less frequent exposure to

    evangelical churches seems to move congregants closer to the Democrats.

    However, lagged partisanship also significantly affects attendance in both eras

    (b = .110 and b = .079, respectively). On average in the 1990s, strong Republicans

    (six on the partisanship scale) increase their attendance to evangelical churches by

    .66 points compared to strong Democrats (zero on the scale). In the 20002004 case,

    strong Republicans increase their attendance to evangelical churches by .47 points

    compared to strong Democrats. As expected therefore, ceteris paribus, the more

    Republican among evangelicals were driven towards participating more frequently

    in their churches, while the more Democratic were motivated to participate less.

    Results support a Republican pull behind increasing attendance in evangelical

    churches (or a Democratic push away from the same churches). Importantly, this

    Table 5 (Partisan) Political religion in recent election cycles

    Evangelical (19921996) Evangelical (20002004)

    Stabilities (Time 1 ? Time 2)

    Party ID ? Party ID .784*** (.786/.064) .903*** (.817/.045)

    Attendance ? Attendance .791*** (.789/.059) .738*** (.752/.049)

    Cross-lags (Time 1 ? Time 2)

    Party ID ? Attendance .110** (.141/.052) .079** (.096/.041)

    Attendance ? Party ID .156** (.121/.072) .247*** (.187/.054)

    Summary statistics

    N 119 200

    v2/df 65.233/47 98.046/47

    (1.388, p = .040) (2.086, p = .000)

    IFI, CFI, RMSEA .976, .973, .057 .960, .957, .074

    Source: 19921996, 20002004 ANES panels

    Note: Maximum likelihood estimates. Parentheses contain standardized coefficients/standard errors.

    Controls included for age, male dummy, education dummies (reference category is high school or less),

    union member in household dummy, marital status dummies (never married is baseline), region dummies

    (northeast is baseline), children in household dummy, and income. Estimates for disturbance correlations,

    unanalyzed relationships, and control effects are omitted for clarity

    * p\ .10; ** p\ .05; *** p\ .01

    384 Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389

    http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    19/24

    phenomenon emerges within the constituency that has been at the forefront in a

    polarized partisan environment.12

    By and large, the causal argument and findings presented above suggest that

    when the link connecting a religious group with an ideological camp or political

    party becomes institutionalized in the public mind, religious behavior in that groupcan be partly shaped by a political calculus. This effect is particularly evident for the

    most salient social group in both the conservative backlash to the social upheaval of

    the 1960s and 1970s, and later in the Republican electoral base: evangelical

    Protestants. I consider the emergence of the ideological effect before the emergence

    of the partisan effect as a robust basis for this conclusion. As a new issue conflict

    emerged in the political scene of the 1970s based on traditional social values,

    ideological political religion was the case for evangelicals in that period (see the

    effect of ideological demarcation in Table 2). Later on, as party elites responded

    and adapted to this new division, partisan political religion appears to have followed(see the effect of partisan demarcation in Table 4) (cf. Carmines and Stimson 1989;

    Layman 2001; Wald and Calhoun-Brown 2007).

    The intense linking of a church with a political ideology or party produces an

    autonomous outcome, which could eventually alter the composition of the religious

    constituency. Even though available datasets can only indirectly support this

    conclusion, it seems that if, for instance, Democrats reduce their attendance to

    evangelical churches, and Republicans increase it, political homogeneity within

    these churches may not be a mere effect of theology or demography.

    Discussion

    The rising voice of evangelical Protestants, mobilized by abortion and other social

    issues in the 1970s, and later expressed by the GOP, is considered today an

    important component of the American political system. The present analysis

    contributes an alternative theoretical and empirical explanation of this phenomenon,

    based on the political reinforcement of religious patterns in American politics. The

    alternative proposed and tested moves beyond reductionist sociostructural accounts

    of the cleavage phenomenon, which merely focus on political consequences of

    religious factors.

    Expanding on Hout and Fischers (2002) empirical demonstration of how

    religious communities become affected by their vocal participation in political

    conflicts, I provide more robust evidence of this transformation, in terms of causal

    inference. Results shed light on the role of ideological and partisan group

    identification in generating changes in the religious behavior of evangelical

    Protestants. In a process more or less ignored by prior research, which insists on

    12 To test whether interactions are significant, i.e. whether effects differ across religious communities,

    I fit two models for each panel: first I allow all parameters to differ across groups. In the second case,

    Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389 385

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    20/24

    investigating the conventional effect of social structure on politics, this analysis

    suggests that ideology and partisanship may be able to construct religious

    communities, by boosting movement within and perhaps eventually across these

    communities.

    Certainly, the results presented here are only a preliminary attempt to address anignored social psychological process. Findings are not without methodological

    difficulties, since the designing teams of ANES surveys have worked under the

    assumption that religion affects politics and not vice versa. More fine-tuned survey

    questions coupled with contextual and other in-depth analyses should delve deeper

    into the reinforcing connection between religiosity and political variables. One

    suggestion in that direction would be the consistent repeated measurement of

    denominational membership for the same individuals across time, which can assess

    if this indicator is also subject to ideological and partisan influences. Such an

    analysis could establish if changing attendance within politicized churcheseventually leads to apostasy or switching. Further research could also help

    understand the effect of this phenomenon on voting choice. This step can evaluate

    whether political participation and volatility in voting intentions varies between

    believers who are pulled closer to church or pushed further away from it due to

    political concerns.

    Bearing these weaknesses in mind, what does the analysis of panel data tell us

    about religious politicization? The empirical picture suggests that since the 1970s,

    the ideological and partisan mobilization of evangelicals has pulled some members

    closer to the church (especially conservatives and Republicans), while pushingothers away (liberals and Democrats). Worshiping in a theologically conservative

    church seems to eventually functionat least in partas a symbolic expression of

    conservatism and Republican partisanship, whereby, other things being equal,

    conservatives and Republicans tend to attend church because they see this practice

    as confirmation of ideology and partisanship and as demarcation from the out-group

    (liberals and Democrats). One can only speculate at this point, but the long-term

    consequence of this political religion could lead to an ideological and partisan

    sorting-out within politicized churches, a process that should reinforce the trend

    described by Fig. 1.

    Finally, apart from offering a less simplified portrayal of religious politicization,

    the political religion phenomenon has substantial implications for our understanding

    of religion in modern society and the continuing importance of partisan attachments

    in contemporary American politics. In relation to religion, it appears that American

    piety is partly shaped by worldly components. By focusing on surface indicators,

    research usually ignores this phenomenon and instead concludes that secularization

    is evidently not taking place in the United States, with its high religious observance

    and growing religious mobilization in recent political periods (Leege and Kellstedt

    1993). Even so, this article reinforces the possibility that the basis of Christian faith

    in America is exposed to secular concerns (cf. Bellah 1967).

    Regarding parties, the set of empirical findings presented opens up the potential

    386 Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    21/24

    et al. 2002; Greene 2004), but that they may affect non-political aspects of social

    life as well. In the end, contradicting the proposition that parties are in decline (Nie

    et al. 1979; Aldrich 1995), and that partisanship is merely shorthand for policy

    preferences and short-term evaluations (Fiorina 1981), the existence of a political

    religion supports a view of party identification as a major component of mass beliefsystems.

    Acknowledgments Earlier versions of the paper were presented at the Joint Sessions of the European

    Consortium for Political Research (Workshop: Politicizing Socio-Cultural Structures), Helsinki, May

    2007, and the annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Religion, Tampa, November 2007.

    I am indebted to Mark Shephard, John Curtice, Robert Johns, the editors and two anonymous reviewers

    for advice and comments. I also want to thank Christopher J. Carman and Wouter van der Brug for

    helpful suggestions.

    References

    Abramowitz, A., & Saunders, K. (1998). Ideological realignment in the U.S. electorate. Journal of

    Politics, 60, 634652.

    Aldrich, J. H. (1995). Why parties? The origin and transformation of party politics in America. Chicago:

    University of Chicago Press.

    Arbuckle, J. (2005). Amos 6.0 users guide. Chicago, IL: SmallWaters Corporation.

    Asher, H. (1983). Causal modeling. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Bartels, L. M. (1999). Panel effects in the American national election studies. Political Analysis, 8, 120.

    Bellah, R. (1967). Civil religion in America. Daedalus, 96, 121.

    Bolzendahl, C., & Brooks, C. (2005). Polarization, secularization or differences as usual? The

    denominational cleavage in U.S. social attitudes since the 1970s. Sociological Quarterly, 46, 4778.

    Bollen, K. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.

    Campbell, A. (1960). Surge and decline: A study of electoral change. Public Opinion Quarterly, 24,

    397418.

    Campbell, A., Converse, P., Miller, W., & Stokes, D. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley.

    Campbell, A., Converse, P., Miller, W., & Stokes, D. (1966). Elections & the political order. NY: Wiley.

    Carmines, E., & Stimson, J. (1989). Issue evolution. Princeton: Princeton UP.

    Carsey, T., & Layman, G. (2006). Changing sides or changing minds? Party identification and policy

    preferences in the American electorate. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 464477.

    Clarke, H., & Stewart, M. (1998). The decline of parties in the minds of citizens. Annual Review of

    Political Science, 1, 357378.

    Davidson, R., & Oleszek, W. (2004). Congress and its members (9th ed.). Washington: CQ Press.Deaux, K, Reid, A., Mizrahi, K., & Ethier K. (1995). Parameters of social identity. Journal of Personality

    and Social Psychology, 68, 280291.

    Edelman, M. (1964). The symbolic uses of politics. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

    Finke, R., & Stark, R. (1992). The churching of America, 17761990: Winners and losers in our religious

    economy. New Brunswick: Rutgers UP.

    Finkel, S. (1995). Causal analysis with panel data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Fiorina, M. (1981). Retrospective voting in American national elections. Yale UP.

    Goren, P. (2005). Party identification and core political values. American Journal of Political Science, 49,

    881896.

    Green, D., & Palmquist, B. (1990). Of artifacts and partisan instability. American Journal of Political

    Science, 34, 872902.

    Green, D., Palmquist B., & Schickler, E. (2002). Partisan hearts and minds: Political parties and thesocial identities of voters. New Haven: Yale UP.

    Green J C Rozell M & Wilcox C (Eds ) (2003) The Christian right in American politics: marching

    Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389 387

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    22/24

    Greene, S. (2002). The social-psychological measurement of partisanship. Political Behavior, 24,

    171197.

    Greene, S. (2004). Social identity theory and party identification. Social Science Quarterly, 85, 136153.

    Guth, J. L., Green, J. C., Smidt, C., Kellstedt, L., & Poloma, M. (1997). The bully pulpit: The politics of

    Protestant clergy. Lawrence: University of Kansas Press.

    Hadaway, K., Marler, P. L., & Chaves, M. (1993). What the polls dont show: A closer look at churchattendance. American Sociological Review, 58, 741752.

    Hogg, M. A., & Terry, D. J. (2000). Social identity and self-categorization processes in organizational

    contexts. Academy of Management Review, 25, 121140.

    Hout, M., & Fischer, C. (2002). Why more Americans have no religious preference. American

    Sociological Review, 67, 165190.

    Huddy, L. (2003). Group identity and political cohesion. In D. O. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.),

    Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford UP.

    Jackson, J. E. (1975). Issues, party choices and presidential votes. American Journal of Political Science,

    19, 161186.

    Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. NY: Guilford.

    Kriesi, H. (1998). The transformation of cleavage politics. The 1997 Stein Rokkan Lecture. European

    Journal of Political Research, 33, 165185.

    Lau, R. (1989). Individual and contextual influences on group identification. Social Psychology

    Quarterly, 52, 220231.

    Layman, G. (1997). Religion and political behavior in the United States: The impact of beliefs,

    affiliations, and commitment from 1980 to 1994. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 61, 288316.

    Layman, G. (2001). The great divide: Religious and cultural conflict in American party politics . New

    York: Columbia UP.

    Layman, G., & Carsey, T. (2002). Party polarization and party structuring of policy attitudes: A

    comparison of three NES panel studies. Political Behavior, 24, 199236.

    Layman, G., & Green, J. C. (2005). Wars and rumours of wars: The contexts of cultural conflict in

    American political behaviour. British Journal of Political Science, 36, 6189.

    Layman, G., & Hussey, L. (2005). George W. Bush and the Evangelicals. Presented at the Conference onA Matter of Faith? Religion and the 2004 Presidential Election, University of Notre Dame.

    Leege, D. C., & Kellstedt, L. A. (Eds.). (1993). Rediscovering the religious factor in American politics.

    Armonk, N.Y.: M.E. Sharpe.

    Lenski, G. (1963). The Religious Factor. NY: Doubleday.

    Levendusky, M. (2005). Sorting in the U.S. mass electorate. Presented at the MPSA Annual Meeting.

    Lipset, S. M., & Rokkan, S. (1967). Cleavage structures, party systems, and voter alignments: An

    introduction. In S. M. Lipset, & S. Rokkan (Eds.), Party systems and voter alignments. NY: Free

    Press.

    Long, K. M., & Spears, R. (1997). The self-esteem hypothesis revisited: Differentiation and the

    disaffected. In R. Spears, P. Oakes, N. Ellemers, & S. Haslam (Eds.), The social psychology of

    stereotyping and group life. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Loveland, M. T. (2003). Religious switching: Preference development, maintenance, and change. Journalfor the Scientific Study of Religion, 42, 147157.

    Manza, J., & Brooks, C. (1999). Social cleavages and political change. Oxford: Oxford UP.

    Markus, G, & Converse, P. (1979). A dynamic simultaneous equation model of electoral choice.

    American Political Science Review, 73, 10551070.

    Miller, W. E., & Shanks, J. M. (1996). The new American voter. Cambridge: Harvard UP.

    Moen, M. C. (1994). From revolution to evolution: The changing nature of the Christian right. Sociology

    of Religion, 55, 345357.

    Newport, F. (1979). The religious switcher in the US. American Sociological Review, 44, 528552.

    Nie, N., Verba, S., & Petrocik, J. (1979). The changing American voter. Cambridge: Harvard UP.

    Page, B., & Jones, C. (1979). Reciprocal effects of policy preferences, party loyalties, and the vote.

    American Political Science Review,

    73, 10711090.Poole, K. T., & Rosenthal, H. (1984). The polarization of American politics. Journal of Politics, 46,

    10611079.

    388 Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389

  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    23/24

    Smith, R., Ager, J., & Williams, D. (1992). Suppressor variables in multiple regression/correlation.

    Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52, 1729.

    Steensland, B., Park, J., Regnerus, M., Robinson, L., Wilcox, W. B., & Woodberry, R. (2000). The

    measure of American religion: Toward improving the state of the art. Social Forces, 79, 291318.

    Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.

    Turner, J. C. (1985). Social categorization and the self-concept: A social cognitive theory of groupbehavior. In E. Lawler (Ed.), Advances in group processes: Theory and research Vol. 2. Greenwich,

    CT: JAI Press.

    Wald, K., & Calhoun-Brown, A. (2007). Religion and politics in the United States (5th ed.). Lanham,

    MD: Rowman & Littlefield.

    Warner, R. S. (1993). Work in progress toward a new paradigm for the sociological study of religion in

    the United States. American Journal of Sociology, 98, 10441093.

    Wilcox, C. (1996). Onward Christian soldiers: The religious right in American politics. Boulder, CO:

    Westview.

    Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge UP.

    Polit Behav (2008) 30:367389 389

  • 7/28/2019 33436886

    24/24